Detecting Deception and Judge’s Involvement: Lower Task Involvement Leads to Better Lie Detection

Videotaped samples of target individuals making honest or dishonest statements were shown to 66 male and female participants who judged each sample in terms of honesty. The participants were either highly involved in the judging task or relatively uninvolved. It was assumed that highly involved participants would engage in central route processing and therefore attend more to the verbal message, whereas less-involved participants would engage in peripheral route processing and therefore attend more to the nonverbal behavior of the targets. Because nonverbal cues are most indicative of deception, it was hypothesized—and found—that low-involvement participants would be more accurate at detecting deception than would high-involvement participants. Furthermore, gender differences and support for a motivational impairment effect were found, in which lies told by people who were highly motivated to lie successfully were more easily detected.

[1]  S. Chaiken Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. , 1980 .

[2]  Robert E. Kraut,et al.  Humans as Lie Detectors , 1980 .

[3]  Gerald R. Miller,et al.  Explanations for Visual Cue Primacy in Judgments of Honesty and Deceit , 1989 .

[4]  R. Koestner,et al.  Learning to detect deception. , 1984 .

[5]  S. Fein Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias , 1996 .

[6]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Spotting Lies: Can Humans Learn to Do Better? , 1994 .

[7]  C. Keating,et al.  PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN " Dominance and Deception in Children and Adults : Are Leaders the Best Misleaders ? , 2006 .

[8]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Social anxiety and social judgment: the example of detecting deception , 1994 .

[9]  Franziska Marquart,et al.  Communication and persuasion : central and peripheral routes to attitude change , 1988 .

[10]  G. Daniel Lassiter,et al.  Attentional Determinants of Success at Detecting Deception and Truth , 1982 .

[11]  R. Rosenthal,et al.  Encoding and decoding nonverbal cues of emotion. , 1975, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[12]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  Lie experts' beliefs about nonverbal indicators of deception , 1996 .

[13]  Bernard Rimé,et al.  Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior , 1991 .

[14]  B. Depaulo,et al.  The motivational impairment effect in the communication of deception: Replications and extensions , 1988 .

[15]  Steven Fein Effects of suspicion on attributional thinking and the correspondence bias , 1996 .

[16]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Effects of actual deception and suspiciousness of deception on interpersonal perceptions. , 1984 .

[17]  L. Zebrowitz,et al.  "Wide-Eyed" and "Crooked-Faced": Determinants of Perceived and Real Honesty Across the Life Span , 1996 .

[18]  Michael Lewis,et al.  Lying and deception in everyday life , 1993 .

[19]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Telling lies. , 1979, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  R. Rosenthal,et al.  Nonverbal strategies for decoding deception , 1982 .

[21]  M. Zuckerman Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception , 1981 .

[22]  B. Depaulo,et al.  Telling ingratiating lies: effects of target sex and target attractiveness on verbal and nonverbal deceptive success. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  P. Ekman,et al.  Who can catch a liar? , 1991, The American psychologist.

[24]  Jennifer A. Epstein,et al.  Sex differences in lying: How women and men deal with the dilemma of deceit. , 1993 .

[25]  B. Depaulo,et al.  The Motivational Impairment Effect in the Communication of Deception , 1989 .

[26]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Approaches , 1981 .