Neural changes in the primate brain correlated with the evolution of complex motor skills

Complex motor skills of eventual benefit can be learned after considerable trial and error. What do structural brain changes that accompany such effortful long-term learning tell us about the mechanisms for developing innovative behavior? Using MRI, we monitored brain structure before, during and after four marmosets learnt to use a rake, over a long period of 10–13 months. Throughout learning, improvements in dexterity and visuo-motor co-ordination correlated with increased volume in the lateral extrastriate cortex. During late learning, when the most complex behavior was maintained by sustained motivation to acquire the skill, the volume of the nucleus accumbens increased. These findings reflect the motivational state required to learn, and show accelerated function in higher visual cortex that is consistent with neurocognitive divergence across a spectrum of primate species.

[1]  S. Malaivijitnond,et al.  Marine prey processed with stone tools by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea) in intertidal habitats. , 2012, American journal of physical anthropology.

[2]  J. Salamone,et al.  The Mysterious Motivational Functions of Mesolimbic Dopamine , 2012, Neuron.

[3]  S. Ichinose,et al.  Extension of Corticocortical Afferents into the Anterior Bank of the Intraparietal Sulcus by Tool-use Training in Adult Monkeys , 2005 .

[4]  Tadashi Isa,et al.  Function of the nucleus accumbens in motor control during recovery after spinal cord injury , 2015, Science.

[5]  T. Furuichi,et al.  Chimpanzees and bonobos differ in intrinsic motivation for tool use , 2015, Scientific Reports.

[6]  Michael Petrides,et al.  The marmoset brain in stereotaxic coordinates , 2012 .

[7]  Russell D Gray,et al.  The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  T. Asamizuya,et al.  Gray and white matter changes associated with tool-use learning in macaque monkeys , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  Leo L. Lui,et al.  Structure and function of the middle temporal visual area (MT) in the marmoset: Comparisons with the macaque monkey , 2015, Neuroscience Research.

[10]  Leo L. Lui,et al.  Breaking camouflage: responses of neurons in the middle temporal area to stimuli defined by coherent motion , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[11]  Min-Ying Su,et al.  Application of an automated voxel-based morphometry technique to assess regional gray and white matter brain atrophy in a canine model of aging , 2006, NeuroImage.

[12]  Gabriele Ende,et al.  The hippocampus and exercise: histological correlates of MR-detected volume changes , 2014, Brain Structure and Function.

[13]  E. Deci,et al.  Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. , 2000, The American psychologist.

[14]  T. Matsuzawa,et al.  Invention and modification of a new tool use behavior: ant‐fishing in trees by a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea , 2008, American journal of primatology.

[15]  Atsushi Iriki,et al.  Population-averaged standard template brain atlas for the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) , 2011, NeuroImage.

[16]  R. Krauzlis,et al.  Superior colliculus and visual spatial attention. , 2013, Annual review of neuroscience.

[17]  Atsushi Iriki,et al.  Sustained performance by common marmosets in a delayed matching to position task with variable stimulus presentations , 2016, Behavioural Brain Research SreeTestContent1.

[18]  Tadashi Isa,et al.  Neural Substrates for the Motivational Regulation of Motor Recovery after Spinal-Cord Injury , 2011, PloS one.

[19]  Bogdan Draganski,et al.  Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training , 2004, Nature.

[20]  Yumiko Yamazaki,et al.  Marmosets as a next-generation model of comparative cognition , 2009 .

[21]  Judith M Burkart,et al.  Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Coding of modified body schema during tool use by macaque postcentral neurones. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[23]  M. Kage A Critical Review of Studies On Intrinsic Motivation , 1994 .

[24]  Jon H Kaas,et al.  The evolution of neocortex in primates. , 2012, Progress in brain research.

[25]  E. Peterhans,et al.  Neurons in monkey visual cortex detect lines defined by coherent motion of dots , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[26]  G. Elston,et al.  Visuotopic organisation and neuronal response selectivity for direction of motion in visual areas of the caudal temporal lobe of the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): Middle temporal area, middle temporal crescent, and surrounding cortex , 1998, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[27]  M. Rosa Visual maps in the adult primate cerebral cortex: some implications for brain development and evolution. , 2002, Brazilian journal of medical and biological research = Revista brasileira de pesquisas medicas e biologicas.

[28]  D. C. Henckel,et al.  Case report. , 1995, Journal.

[29]  R. Lemon Descending pathways in motor control. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[30]  E. Visalberghi,et al.  Cross‐genus adoption of a marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) by wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus): case report , 2006, American journal of primatology.

[31]  L. Jäncke,et al.  The rewarding value of good motor performance in the context of monetary incentives , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  Tool-use learning by common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.