Relationship between velocity and curvature of a human locomotor trajectory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. Viviani,et al. The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. , 1983, Acta psychologica.
[2] P Viviani,et al. Segmentation and coupling in complex movements. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] J. F. Soechting,et al. Coordination of arm movements in three-dimensional space. Sensorimotor mapping during drawing movement , 1986, Neuroscience.
[4] P. Viviani,et al. SPATIAL CODING OF VOLUNTARY SACCADES IN MAN , 1987 .
[5] A. Wing,et al. Relation between velocity and curvature in movement: equivalence and divergence between a power law and a minimum-jerk model. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] P. Viviani,et al. The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[7] R. Klatzky,et al. Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[8] P. Viviani,et al. A developmental study of the relationship between geometry and kinematics in drawing movements. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] Stefan Glasauer,et al. Idiothetic navigation in Gerbils and Humans , 1991 .
[10] P. Viviani,et al. Biological movements look uniform: evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] R. Klatzky,et al. Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[12] T. Flash,et al. Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] D. Ostry,et al. Origins of the power law relation between movement velocity and curvature: modeling the effects of muscle mechanics and limb dynamics. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] A Berthoz,et al. The predictive brain: anticipatory control of head direction for the steering of locomotion , 1996, Neuroreport.
[15] Gabriel Baud-Bovy,et al. Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[16] A. Berthoz,et al. Circular trajectory formation during blind locomotion: a test for path integration and motor memory , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. Making smooth moves , 2022 .
[18] R Plamondon,et al. The 2/3 power law: when and why? , 1998, Acta psychologica.
[19] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Smoothness maximization along a predefined path accurately predicts the speed profiles of complex arm movements. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] S. Schaal,et al. Segmentation of endpoint trajectories does not imply segmented control , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[21] V. V. Marlinsky,et al. Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man—III. Route inference , 1999, Neuroscience.
[22] G. Cheron,et al. Kinematics invariance in multi-directional complex movements in free space: effect of changing initial direction , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[23] S. Schaal,et al. Origins and violations of the 2/3 power law in rhythmic three-dimensional arm movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.