Relationship between velocity and curvature of a human locomotor trajectory

[1]  P. Viviani,et al.  The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[2]  P Viviani,et al.  Segmentation and coupling in complex movements. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Coordination of arm movements in three-dimensional space. Sensorimotor mapping during drawing movement , 1986, Neuroscience.

[4]  P. Viviani,et al.  SPATIAL CODING OF VOLUNTARY SACCADES IN MAN , 1987 .

[5]  A. Wing,et al.  Relation between velocity and curvature in movement: equivalence and divergence between a power law and a minimum-jerk model. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  P. Viviani,et al.  The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[7]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  P. Viviani,et al.  A developmental study of the relationship between geometry and kinematics in drawing movements. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in Gerbils and Humans , 1991 .

[10]  P. Viviani,et al.  Biological movements look uniform: evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  T. Flash,et al.  Minimum-jerk, two-thirds power law, and isochrony: converging approaches to movement planning. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  D. Ostry,et al.  Origins of the power law relation between movement velocity and curvature: modeling the effects of muscle mechanics and limb dynamics. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A Berthoz,et al.  The predictive brain: anticipatory control of head direction for the steering of locomotion , 1996, Neuroreport.

[15]  Gabriel Baud-Bovy,et al.  Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Circular trajectory formation during blind locomotion: a test for path integration and motor memory , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Making smooth moves , 2022 .

[18]  R Plamondon,et al.  The 2/3 power law: when and why? , 1998, Acta psychologica.

[19]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Smoothness maximization along a predefined path accurately predicts the speed profiles of complex arm movements. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  S. Schaal,et al.  Segmentation of endpoint trajectories does not imply segmented control , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  V. V. Marlinsky,et al.  Vestibular and vestibulo-proprioceptive perception of motion in the horizontal plane in blindfolded man—III. Route inference , 1999, Neuroscience.

[22]  G. Cheron,et al.  Kinematics invariance in multi-directional complex movements in free space: effect of changing initial direction , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[23]  S. Schaal,et al.  Origins and violations of the 2/3 power law in rhythmic three-dimensional arm movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.