Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear

This paper applies the behavior systems approach to fear and defensive behavior, examining the neural circuitry controlling fear and defensive behavior from this vantage point. The defensive behavior system is viewed as having three modes that are activated by different levels of fear. Low levels of fear promote pre-encounter defenses, such as meal-pattern reorganization. Moderate levels of fear activate post-encounter defenses. For the rat, freezing is the dominant post-encounter defensive response. Since this mode of defense is activated by learned fear, forebrain structures such as the amygdala play a critical role in its organization. Projections from the amygdala to the ventral periaqueductal gray activate freezing. Extremely high levels of fear, such as those provoked by physical contact, elicit the vigorous active defenses that compose the circa-strike mode. Midbrain structures such as the dorsolateral periaqueductal gray and the superior colliculus play a crucial role in organizing this mode of defense. Inhibitory interactions between the structures mediating circa-strike and post-encounter defense allow for the rapid switching between defensive modes as the threatening situation varies.

[1]  E. Tolman Purposive behavior in animals and men , 1932 .

[2]  B. Skinner,et al.  Some quantitative properties of anxiety , 1941 .

[3]  M. Lyon The role of central midbrain structures in conditioned responding to aversive noise in the rat , 1964, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[4]  R. Bolles Theory of Motivation , 1967 .

[5]  D C Blanchard,et al.  Crouching as an index of fear. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[6]  R. Bolles Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. , 1970 .

[7]  J. Liebman,et al.  Mesencephalic central gray lesions and fear-motivated behavior in rats. , 1970, Brain research.

[8]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Innate and conditioned reactions to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[9]  R. Bolles,et al.  Freezing as an avoidance response: Another look at the operant-respondent distinction☆ , 1973 .

[10]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Environmental control of defensive reactions to footshock , 1976 .

[11]  T. C. Bond Theory of Motivation, 2nd ed , 1976 .

[12]  R. Bolles,et al.  The effect of predictive cues on freezing in rats , 1976 .

[13]  R. Bolles,et al.  Clinical implications of Bolles & Fanselow's pain/fear model , 1980, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[14]  R. Bolles,et al.  On the Ability of Prey to Recognize Predators , 1980 .

[15]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Conditional and unconditional components of post-shock freezing , 1980, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[16]  J. Liebeskind,et al.  Evidence for opioid and non-opioid forms of stimulation-produced analgesia in the rat , 1982, Brain Research.

[17]  M. Fanselow The postshock activity burst , 1982 .

[18]  L. Watkins,et al.  Classical conditioning of front paw and hind paw footshock induced analgesia (FSIA): Naloxone reversibility and descending pathways , 1982, Brain Research.

[19]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Conditioned fear-induced opiate analgesia on the Formalin test: Evidence for two aversive motivational systems , 1982 .

[20]  R. Bolles,et al.  CS modality, context conditioning, and conditioned freezing , 1983 .

[21]  A Sakaguchi,et al.  Subcortical efferent projections of the medial geniculate nucleus mediate emotional responses conditioned to acoustic stimuli , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Grau Influence of naloxone on shock-induced freezing and analgesia. , 1984, Behavioral neuroscience.

[23]  Opiate modulation of the active and inactive components of the postshock reaction : parallels between naloxone pretreatment and shock intensity , 1984 .

[24]  J. Ayres,et al.  Concurrent observations of barpress suppression and freezing: Effects of CS modality and on-line vs. off-line training upon posttrial behavior , 1985 .

[25]  R. N. Leaton,et al.  Potentiated Startle: Its Relation to Freezing and Shock Intensity in Rats , 1985 .

[26]  M. Fanselow Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: Implications for the response selection rules governing species-specific defensive reactions ☆ , 1986 .

[27]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Effects of naltrexone on learning and performance of conditional fear-induced freezing and opioid analgesia , 1987, Physiology & Behavior.

[28]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Response selection and the hierarchical organization of species-specific defense reactions: the relationship between freezing, flight, and defensive burying , 1987 .

[29]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  Michael S. Fanselow,et al.  A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. , 1988 .

[31]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Contextual conditioning with massed versus distributed unconditional stimuli in the absence of explicit conditional stimuli. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[32]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Changes in feeding and foraging patterns as an antipredator defensive strategy: a laboratory simulation using aversive stimulation in a closed economy. , 1988, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[33]  R. Bandler,et al.  Elicitation of intraspecific defence reactions in the rat from midbrain periaqueductal grey by microinjection of kainic acid, without neurotoxic effects , 1988, Neuroscience Letters.

[34]  M. Fanselow The adaptive function of conditioned defensive behavior: an ecological approach to Pavlovian stimulus-substitution theory , 1989 .

[35]  R. Bandler,et al.  Somatic and autonomic integration in the midbrain of the unanesthetized decerebrate cat: a distinctive pattern evoked by excitation of neurones in the subtentorial portion of the midbrain periaqueductal grey , 1989, Brain Research.

[36]  P. Dean,et al.  Event or emergency? Two response systems in the mammalian superior colliculus , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  D. Blanchard,et al.  An ethoexperimental approach to the study of defense , 1989 .

[38]  S. Maier Determinants of the nature of environmentally induced hypoalgesia. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[39]  R. Bandler,et al.  Characterization of pretentorial periaqueductal gray matter neurons mediating intraspecific defensive behaviors in the rat by microinjections of kainic acid , 1989, Brain Research.

[40]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Synaptic plasticity in fear conditioning circuits: induction of LTP in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala by stimulation of the medial geniculate body , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[41]  M. Fanselow Factors governing one-trial contextual conditioning , 1990 .

[42]  F. Helmstetter,et al.  Conditional hypoalgesia is attenuated by Naltrexone applied to the periaqueductal gray , 1990, Brain Research.

[43]  M. Fanselow The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray as a Coordinator of Action in Response to Fear and Anxiety , 1991 .

[44]  M Ennis,et al.  Connections between the central nucleus of the amygdala and the midbrain periaqueductal gray: Topography and reciprocity , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[45]  Peter Redgrave,et al.  Does the PAG Learn about Emergencies from the Superior Colliculus , 1991 .

[46]  A. Craig,et al.  Organization of Spinal and Trigeminal Input to the PAG , 1991 .

[47]  T. Lovick Interactions Between Descending Pathways from the Dorsal and Ventrolateral Periaqueductal Gray Matter in the Rat , 1991 .

[48]  U. Jürgens Neurochemical Study of PAG Control of Vocal Behavior , 1991 .

[49]  Charles R. Larson,et al.  Activity of PAG Neurons During Conditioned Vocalization in the Macaque Monkey , 1991 .

[50]  D. C. Blanchard,et al.  Twenty-two kHz alarm cries to presentation of a predator, by laboratory rats living in visible burrow systems , 1991, Physiology & Behavior.

[51]  M. T. Shipley,et al.  Topographical Specificity of Forebrain Inputs to the Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray: Evidence for Discrete Longitudinally Organized Input Columns , 1991 .

[52]  R. Bandler,et al.  Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray Control of Defensive Behavior in the Cat and the Rat , 1991 .

[53]  R. Bandler,et al.  Integration of somatic and autonomic reactions within the midbrain periaqueductal grey: viscerotopic, somatotopic and functional organization. , 1991, Progress in brain research.

[54]  F. Helmstetter,et al.  The amygdala is essential for the expression of conditional hypoalgesia. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[55]  R. Baker,et al.  The pathways and functions of GABA in the oculomotor system. , 1992, Progress in brain research.

[56]  Michael Davis,et al.  The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear. , 1992 .

[57]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. , 1992, Science.

[58]  Joseph E. LeDoux,et al.  Emotion and the amygdala. , 1992 .

[59]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Associative regulation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: unconditional stimulus intensity, incentive shifts, and latent inhibition. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[60]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Differential Contribution of Amygdala and Hippocampus to Cued and Contextual Fear Conditioning , 1992 .

[61]  Paul J. Whalen,et al.  Amygdaloid contributions to conditioned arousal and sensory information processing. , 1992 .

[62]  M. Fanselow,et al.  The benzodiazepine inverse agonist DMCM as an unconditional stimulus for fear-induced analgesia: implications for the role of GABAA receptors in fear-related behavior. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[63]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Effects of amygdala, hippocampus, and periaqueductal gray lesions on short- and long-term contextual fear. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[64]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Aversively motivated changes in meal patterns of rats in a closed economy: The effects of shock density , 1993 .

[65]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Mechanisms responsible for reduced contextual conditioning with massed unsignaled unconditional stimuli. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[66]  W. Timberlake Behavior systems and reinforcement: an integrative approach. , 1993, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[67]  J. Yeomans,et al.  Amygdala efferents mediating electrically evoked startle-like responses and fear potentiation of acoustic startle. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[68]  M. Fanselow,et al.  NMDA processes mediate anterograde amnesia of contextual fear conditioning induced by hippocampal damage: immunization against amnesia by context preexposure. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[69]  M. Fanselow,et al.  The immediate-shock deficit and postshock analgesia: Implications for the relationship between the analgesic CR and UR , 1994 .

[70]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Acquisition of contextual Pavlovian fear conditioning is blocked by application of an NMDA receptor antagonist D,L-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid to the basolateral amygdala. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[71]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Ventral and dorsolateral regions of the midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG) control different stages of defensive behavior: Dorsolateral PAG lesions enhance the defensive freezing produced by massed and immediate shock , 1995 .