The Cognitive Style of Breadth of Categorization : Longitudinal Consistency of Personality Correlates

Four tasks measuring breadth of categorization in different ways were administered to 128 4-year-old children. Independent personality data were available on these children at ages 3,4, and 7 in the form of behavior ratings by independent sets of teachers using the California Child Q-Set and in the form of behavior ratings by independent sets of examiners. The four category breadth measures were standard-scored and composited to generate a domain-representative index of category breadth. This index was then correlated with the independent personality ratings available at each of the four ages. A core set of relationships, common across both age and sex, indicated that broad categorizers, relative to narrow categorizers, had difficulty in stimulus exclusion, lacked autonomous structuring, and generally behaved with few internal constraints. Boys were significantly broader in their categorization than girls, although there was appreciable overlap between the sexes. Personality correlates of breadth of categorization observed as early as 3 years of age were also observed at age 7. Results were placed in a larger theoretical context by introducing the concept of underand overperceptualization.

[1]  Louis Guttman,et al.  A basis for analyzing test-retest reliability , 1945, Psychometrika.

[2]  Samuel J. Beck,et al.  The Q-Sort Method in Personality Assessment and Psychiatric Research. , 1962 .

[3]  R. Cattell,et al.  Vehicles, ipsatization, and the multiple-method measurement of motivation. , 1965, Canadian journal of psychology.

[4]  Bert F. Green,et al.  In defense of measurement. , 1978 .

[5]  J. Loevinger,et al.  The attenuation paradox in test theory. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.

[6]  P. Lazarsfeld Principles of Topological Psychology , 1938 .

[7]  Paul McReynolds,et al.  A Restricted Conceptualization of Human Anxiety and Motivation , 1956 .

[8]  J. Block The equivalence of measures and the correction for attenuation. , 1963, Psychological bulletin.

[9]  Eli Saltz,et al.  The cognitive bases of human learning , 1971 .

[10]  L. Cronbach Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests , 1951 .

[11]  I. Sigel,et al.  Concept overdiscrimination in children. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  Seymour Epstein,et al.  The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time. , 1979 .

[13]  J. Bruner On perceptual readiness. , 1957, Psychological review.

[14]  P. McReynolds The Rorschach concept evaluation technique. , 1954, Journal of projective techniques.

[15]  J. Block,et al.  The Role of Ego-Control and Ego-Resiliency in the Organization of Behavior , 1980 .

[16]  Riley W. Gardner,et al.  Differentiation and abstraction in concept formation. , 1962 .

[17]  J. Block RECOGNIZING ATTENUATION EFFECTS IN THE STRATEGY OF RESEARCH. , 1964, Psychological bulletin.

[18]  J. Block,et al.  Some Misgivings About the Matching Familiar Figures Test as a Measure of Reflection-Impulsivity. , 1974 .

[19]  L. Humphreys Note on the multitrait-multimethod matrix. , 1960, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  J. B. Maller,et al.  Studies in the Nature of Character. , 1930 .

[21]  M. Wallach,et al.  Attribute criteriality and sex-linked conservatism as determinants of psychological similarity. , 1959, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  H. A. Witkin,et al.  Field dependence and interpersonal behavior. , 1976, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  J. Lacey Psychophysiological approaches to the evaluation of psychotherapeutic process and outcome. , 1959 .