The Role of Sublexical Graphemic Processing in Reading

Dual-route models of reading postulate the existence of two separate mechanisms: The lexical route allows words to be recognized in their holistic form, and the sublexical route proceeds by converting the written sublexical entities of a word or a nonword into their corresponding phonological equivalents. Sublexical reading is assumed to require three stages of processing: graphemic parsing, graphophonemic conversion, and phoneme blending. This study provides evidence in favor of the existence of a graphemic parsing process which occurs prior to grapheme-phoneme conversion. A group of normal subjects read nonwords which contained multiletter graphemes significantly more slowly than graphemically simple nonwords. These results, best interpretable in the context of a recent dual-route model of reading, confirm previous data obtained in pathology which suggest the functional independence of this cognitive procedure.

[1]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  PHONOLOGICALLY PLAUSIBLE ERRORS: IMPLICATIONS FOR A MODEL OF THE PHONEME-GRAPHEME CONVERSION MECHANISM IN THE SPELLING PROCESS , 1986 .

[2]  J. Derouesné The "phonemic" stage in the non-lexical reading process: evidence from a case of phonological alexia , 1995 .

[3]  Derek Besner,et al.  This and THAP — Constraints on the Pronunciation of New, Written Words , 1981 .

[4]  E. Funnell Phonological processes in reading: new evidence from acquired dyslexia. , 1983 .

[5]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The role of the (output) phonological buffer in reading, writing, and repetition , 1986 .

[6]  Ulrich H. Frauenfelder,et al.  Bidirectional grapheme-phoneme activation in a bimodal detection task. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Reading mechanisms and the organisation of the lexicon: Evidence from acquired dyslexia , 1985 .

[8]  Rosaleen A. McCarthy,et al.  Reading without Semantics , 1983 .

[9]  K. Paap,et al.  Dual-route models of print to sound: Still a good horse race , 1991 .

[10]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[11]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading. , 1994 .

[12]  A. Caramazza,et al.  The structure of graphemic representations , 1990, Cognition.

[13]  J Dérouesné,et al.  Phonological alexia: three dissociations. , 1979 .

[14]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[15]  M. Rosson From SOFA to LOUCH: Lexical contributions to pseudoword pronunciation , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[16]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[17]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Nonword pronunciation and models of word recognition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  S. Monsell,et al.  Lexical and sublexical translation of spelling to sound : Strategic anticipation of lexical status , 1992 .

[19]  R. Glushko The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .

[20]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Surface dyslexia. , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[21]  A. W. Ellis Reading, Writing and Dyslexia: A Cognitive Analysis , 1984 .

[22]  M. Coltheart Phonological dyslexia: Past and future issues. , 1996 .

[23]  J Dérouesné,et al.  Phonological processing in reading: data from alexia. , 1979 .

[24]  G. Underwood Strategies of information processing , 1980 .

[25]  Janice Kay,et al.  One Process, Not Two, in Reading Aloud: Lexical Analogies Do the Work of Non-Lexical Rules , 1981 .

[26]  M. Coltheart Lexical access in simple reading tasks , 1978 .