Brain Regions Engaged by Part- and Whole-task Performance in a Video Game: A Model-based Test of the Decomposition Hypothesis

Part- and whole-task conditions were created by manipulating the presence of certain components of the Space Fortress video game. A cognitive model was created for two-part games that could be combined into a model that performed the whole game. The model generated predictions both for behavioral patterns and activation patterns in various brain regions. The activation predictions concerned both tonic activation that was constant in these regions during performance of the game and phasic activation that occurred when there was resource competition. The model's predictions were confirmed about how tonic and phasic activation in different regions would vary with condition. These results support the Decomposition Hypothesis that the execution of a complex task can be decomposed into a set of information-processing components and that these components combine unchanged in different task conditions. In addition, individual differences in learning gains were predicted by individual differences in phasic activation in those regions that displayed highest tonic activity. This individual difference pattern suggests that the rate of learning of a complex skill is determined by capacity limits.

[1]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[2]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[3]  Frank J. Lee,et al.  Does Learning a Complex Task Have to Be Complex?: A Study in Learning Decomposition , 2001, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  Niels Taatgen,et al.  The Multitasking Mind , 2010, Oxford series on cognitive models and architectures.

[5]  Walter R. Boot,et al.  Predicting Individuals' Learning Success from Patterns of Pre-Learning MRI Activity , 2011, PloS one.

[6]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional Dissociation among Components of Remembering: Control, Perceived Oldness, and Content , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  J. Brožek Attention and Performance II. , 1971 .

[8]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[9]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  Concurrent Automatic and Controlled Visual Search: Can Processing Occur Without Resource Cost? , 1982 .

[10]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Student modeling in the ACT Programming Tutor. , 1995 .

[11]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[12]  D. Gopher,et al.  Practice under changing priorities: An approach to the training of complex skills☆ , 1989 .

[13]  C. Ferguson The Good, The Bad and the Ugly: A Meta-analytic Review of Positive and Negative Effects of Violent Video Games , 2007, Psychiatric Quarterly.

[14]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[15]  Jeffrey A. Jordan,et al.  A Dyadic Protocol for Training Complex Skills , 1992 .

[16]  J. Adams Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and transfer of human motor skills. , 1987 .

[17]  E. Donchin The learning strategies project: Introductory remarks☆ , 1989 .

[18]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Using fMRI to Test Models of Complex Cognition , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[19]  Florin Dolcos,et al.  Similarities and Differences in the Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory Retrieval and Working Memory , 2002, NeuroImage.

[20]  John R. Anderson,et al.  A central circuit of the mind , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: A computational model , 2001, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  F. Donders On the speed of mental processes. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[23]  S. Sternberg,et al.  The meaning of additive reaction-time effects: tests of three alternatives , 1993 .

[24]  D. Simons,et al.  The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[25]  S. Dehaene,et al.  THREE PARIETAL CIRCUITS FOR NUMBER PROCESSING , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[26]  Lila Davachi,et al.  When Keeping in Mind Supports Later Bringing to Mind: Neural Markers of Phonological Rehearsal Predict Subsequent Remembering , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  G E BRIGGS,et al.  Effects of task complexity and task organization on the relative efficiency of part and whole training methods. , 1963, Journal of experimental psychology.

[28]  N. Meiran,et al.  On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[29]  Andrea Stocco,et al.  The Neural Correlates of Problem States: Testing fMRI Predictions of a Computational Model of Multitasking , 2010, PloS one.

[30]  D. Gopher,et al.  Transfer of Skill from a Computer Game Trainer to Flight , 1994 .

[31]  S. Los,et al.  On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .

[32]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Automated image registration , 1993 .

[33]  M. Farah,et al.  Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Human Symbol Manipulation Within an Integrated Cognitive Architecture , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[35]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric mapping , 2013 .

[36]  S. C. Strother,et al.  Are brain functions really additive? , 1996, NeuroImage.

[37]  G. Barker,et al.  Study design in fMRI: Basic principles , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[38]  M. Posner,et al.  Attentional networks , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[39]  C. Lebiere,et al.  Conditional routing of information to the cortex: a model of the basal ganglia's role in cognitive coordination. , 2010, Psychological review.

[40]  S. Chipman,et al.  Cognitively diagnostic assessment , 1995 .

[41]  R W Cox,et al.  Software tools for analysis and visualization of fMRI data , 1997, NMR in biomedicine.

[42]  Anthony D Wagner,et al.  Assembling and encoding word representations: fMRI subsequent memory effects implicate a role for phonological control , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Automated image registration: I. General methods and intrasubject, intramodality validation. , 1998, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[44]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[45]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Spanning seven orders of magnitude: a challenge for cognitive modeling , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[46]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[47]  John R. Anderson How Can the Human Mind Occur in the Physical Universe , 2007 .

[48]  I. Spence,et al.  Video Games and Spatial Cognition , 2010 .

[49]  M. Just,et al.  Interdependence of Nonoverlapping Cortical Systems in Dual Cognitive Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.

[50]  D. V. von Cramon,et al.  Localization of Executive Functions in Dual-Task Performance with fMRI , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Striatal activation during acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1999, Neuropsychology.

[52]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  How to test for dual-task-specific effects in brain imaging studies — An evaluation of potential analysis methods , 2011, NeuroImage.

[53]  Barbara Y. White,et al.  An approach to training based upon principled task decomposition , 1989 .

[54]  D. Simons,et al.  Striatal volume predicts level of video game skill acquisition. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  Pat Gunn,et al.  A rational account of memory predicts left prefrontal activation during controlled retrieval. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  David Badre,et al.  Semantic retrieval, mnemonic control, and prefrontal cortex. , 2002, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[57]  D E Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.

[58]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[59]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  Peter A. Bandettini,et al.  The respiration response function: The temporal dynamics of fMRI signal fluctuations related to changes in respiration , 2008, NeuroImage.

[61]  Wai-Tat Fu,et al.  Bridging across cognitive training and brain plasticity: a neurally inspired computational model of interactive skill learning. , 2013, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[62]  Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer,et al.  Training Complex Cognitive Skills: A Four-Component Instructional Design Model for Technical Training , 1997 .

[63]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  The Neural Bases of Strategy and Skill in Sentence–Picture Verification , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[64]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[65]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Conditional Routing of Information to the Neocortex: A Network Model of Basal Ganglia Function , 2008 .