The relationship of perceptual discrimination to neural mechanisms of fear generalization

ABSTRACT The generalization of conditioned fear responses has been shown to decrease as a function of perceptual similarity. However, generalization may also extend beyond the perceptual discrimination threshold, ostensibly due to contributions from processes other than perception. Currently the neural mechanisms that mediate perceptual and non‐perceptual aspects of fear generalization are unclear. To investigate this question, we conducted a Pavlovian fear conditioning and generalization experiment, collecting functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), skin conductance and explicit shock likelihood ratings, in 37 healthy subjects. Face stimuli were initially paired (CS+) or not paired (CS—) with an electrical shock. During the generalization phase, responses were measured to the CS+, CS— and a range of CS + ‐to—CS— morphs (generalization stimuli), selected for each participant based on that participant's discrimination ability. Across multiple measurements, we found that fear generalization responses were limited to stimuli that could not be distinguished from the CS + stimulus, thus following a gradient closely linked to perceptual discriminability. These measurements, which were correlated with one another, included skin conductance responses, behavioral ratings, and fMRI responses of anterior insula and superior frontal gyrus. In contrast, responses in areas of the default network, including the posterior cingulate gyrus, angular gyrus and hippocampus, showed a negative generalization function extending to stimuli that were more likely to be distinguished from the CS+. In addition, the generalization gradients of the anterior insula and the behavioral ratings showed some evidence for extension beyond perceptual limits. Taken together, these results suggest that distinct brain areas are involved in perceptual and non‐perceptual components of fear generalization. HIGHLIGHTSStimuli were individualized based on perceptual discrimination abilities.Fear generalization responses generally followed the perceptual discriminability gradient.Areas in the default network showed inverted generalization functions, extending beyond perceptual thresholds.This approach can identify both perceptual and non‐perceptual aspects of fear generalization.

[1]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  A Stable Sparse Fear Memory Trace in Human Amygdala , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  Don't look down: emotional arousal elevates height perception. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[3]  N. Guttman,et al.  Discriminability and stimulus generalization. , 1956, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  S. Sponheim,et al.  Neural Substrates of Overgeneralized Conditioned Fear in PTSD. , 2017, The American journal of psychiatry.

[5]  B. Harrison,et al.  Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies , 2016, Molecular Psychiatry.

[6]  A. Craig,et al.  How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[7]  C. Grillon,et al.  Overgeneralization of conditioned fear as a pathogenic marker of panic disorder. , 2010, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  Bram Vervliet,et al.  Fear Generalization in Humans: Systematic Review and Implications for Anxiety Disorder Research. , 2015, Behavior therapy.

[9]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion , 2012, NeuroImage.

[10]  Dominik R. Bach,et al.  Stimulus-invariant auditory cortex threat encoding during fear conditioning with simple and complex sounds , 2018, NeuroImage.

[11]  B. Vervliet,et al.  Perceptual discrimination in fear generalization: Mechanistic and clinical implications , 2015, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[13]  D. Holt,et al.  Sex differences in the neurobiology of fear conditioning and extinction: a preliminary fMRI study of shared sex differences with stress-arousal circuitry , 2012, Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders.

[14]  Jonathan P. McNulty,et al.  The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: Replication from DCM , 2014, NeuroImage.

[15]  Michael R. Harwell,et al.  Summarizing Monte Carlo Results in Methodological Research: The One- and Two-Factor Fixed Effects ANOVA Cases , 1992 .

[16]  G. Orban,et al.  Default Mode of Brain Function in Monkeys , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Christian Büchel,et al.  The neuronal basis of fear generalization in humans , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  A. Dale,et al.  Whole Brain Segmentation Automated Labeling of Neuroanatomical Structures in the Human Brain , 2002, Neuron.

[19]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Neural networks underlying the metacognitive uncertainty response , 2015, Cortex.

[20]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Generalization in Category Learning: The Roles of Representational and Decisional Uncertainty , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Lund,et al.  Compulsory averaging of crowded orientation signals in human vision , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Joshua M. Carlson,et al.  VENTROMEDIAL PREFRONTAL CORTEX REACTIVITY IS ALTERED IN GENERALIZED ANXIETY DISORDER DURING FEAR GENERALIZATION , 2013, Depression and anxiety.

[24]  K. Dobkins,et al.  Compromised speed discrimination among schizophrenia patients when viewing smooth pursuit targets , 2007, Schizophrenia Research.

[25]  Marisa O. Hollinshead,et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  R. Tootell,et al.  A parametric study of fear generalization to faces and non-face objects: relationship to discrimination thresholds , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  P. Pauli,et al.  Fear conditioning and stimulus generalization in patients with social anxiety disorder. , 2016, Journal of anxiety disorders.

[28]  Daphne J. Holt,et al.  Extinction Memory Is Impaired in Schizophrenia , 2009, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  John-Dylan Haynes,et al.  The Relationship between Perceptual Decision Variables and Confidence in the Human Brain. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  H. Steven Scholte,et al.  Associative Learning Increases Trial-by-Trial Similarity of BOLD-MRI Patterns , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Deakin,et al.  Characterization of a psychophysiological model of classical fear conditioning in healthy volunteers: influence of gender, instruction, personality and placebo , 2005, Psychopharmacology.

[32]  M. Dawson,et al.  The electrodermal system , 2007 .

[33]  S. Rauch,et al.  Fear conditioning and extinction: influence of sex and menstrual cycle in healthy humans. , 2006, Behavioral neuroscience.

[34]  Brent A. Vogt,et al.  Midcingulate cortex: Structure, connections, homologies, functions and diseases , 2016, Journal of Chemical Neuroanatomy.

[35]  Koenraad Van Leemput,et al.  A computational atlas of the hippocampal formation using ex vivo, ultra-high resolution MRI: Application to adaptive segmentation of in vivo MRI , 2015, NeuroImage.

[36]  Michael N. Geuss,et al.  Fear Similarly Alters Perceptual Estimates of and Actions over Gaps , 2016, PloS one.

[37]  Galia Avidan,et al.  Visual Aversive Learning Compromises Sensory Discrimination , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  C. Grillon,et al.  Generalized Anxiety Disorder Is Associated With Overgeneralization of Classically Conditioned Fear , 2014, Biological Psychiatry.

[39]  V. Menon,et al.  Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[40]  A. Holmes Generalisability, random effects and population inference (abstract) , 1998 .

[41]  T. Endestad,et al.  Evidence Accumulation and Choice Maintenance Are Dissociated in Human Perceptual Decision Making , 2015, PloS one.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Generalisability, Random Effects & Population Inference , 1998, NeuroImage.

[43]  B. Vervliet,et al.  Conditioned Fear Acquisition and Generalization in Generalized Anxiety Disorder. , 2015, Behavior therapy.

[44]  Eric Achten,et al.  Anticipatory processes in brain state switching — Evidence from a novel cued-switching task implicating default mode and salience networks , 2014, NeuroImage.

[45]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  Anterior insula activations in perceptual paradigms: often observed but barely understood , 2010, Brain Structure and Function.

[47]  Moser Edvard,et al.  Pattern Separation in the Dentate Gyrus , 2009 .

[48]  D. Beidel,et al.  Offspring of anxious parents: reactivity, habituation, and anxiety-proneness. , 2005, Behaviour research and therapy.

[49]  Stefania Favilla,et al.  Ranking brain areas encoding the perceived level of pain from fMRI data , 2014, NeuroImage.

[50]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[51]  Kevin S LaBar,et al.  Sex, stress, and fear: Individual differences in conditioned learning , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[52]  Katja Wiech,et al.  Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[53]  R. Edelberg,et al.  Scoring criteria for response latency and habituation in electrodermal research: a critique. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[54]  K. Wiech,et al.  Anterior Insula Integrates Information about Salience into Perceptual Decisions about Pain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[55]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data , 2011, Nature Methods.

[56]  Rony Paz,et al.  Fear Generalization and Anxiety: Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms , 2015, Biological Psychiatry.

[57]  Vishnu P. Murty,et al.  Neurobehavioral mechanisms of human fear generalization , 2011, NeuroImage.

[58]  R. Paz,et al.  Negative Valence Widens Generalization of Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[59]  Joshua M. Carlson,et al.  Neural reactivity tracks fear generalization gradients , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[60]  M. Milad,et al.  Contribution of estradiol levels and hormonal contraceptives to sex differences within the fear network during fear conditioning and extinction , 2015, BMC Psychiatry.

[61]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Domain General Mechanisms of Perceptual Decision Making in Human Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  U. Jürgens,et al.  The efferent and afferent connections of the supplementary motor area , 1984, Brain Research.

[63]  S. Rauch,et al.  Context modulation of memory for fear extinction in humans. , 2005, Psychophysiology.

[64]  Rony Paz,et al.  Auditory aversive learning increases discrimination thresholds , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  V. Menon,et al.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[66]  S. Ghirlanda,et al.  A century of generalization , 2003, Animal Behaviour.

[67]  R. Paz,et al.  Monetary Loss Alters Perceptual Thresholds and Compromises Future Decisions via Amygdala and Prefrontal Networks , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[68]  Valerie L. Kinner,et al.  Let's talk about sex … differences in human fear conditioning , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[69]  C. Korn,et al.  Psychophysiological modeling: Current state and future directions. , 2018, Psychophysiology.

[70]  Jian Kong,et al.  Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[71]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[72]  Jeanine K. Stefanucci,et al.  A new mode of fear expression: perceptual bias in height fear. , 2008, Emotion.

[73]  P. Gasquoine,et al.  Contributions of the Insula to Cognition and Emotion , 2014, Neuropsychology Review.

[74]  Christian Grillon,et al.  Neural substrates of classically conditioned fear-generalization in humans: a parametric fMRI study. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[75]  C. Kennard,et al.  Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[76]  Manfred Fahle,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of perceptual learning in the human brain , 2005, Brain Topography.