Posterior parietal cortex and visuospatial control in near and far space

Neuropsychological studies of patients with visuospatial neglect have shown differences in perceptual deficits for information in near space (i.e. near to the body) and information in far space. It has been suggested that among the many areas of the human brain, a number of areas are associated with a set of spatial maps specialized for visuospatial control related to this spatial distinction. This paper reviews how parietal cortex is thought to be involved in visuospatial neglect in relation to its control of visuospatial attention in the left and right visual fields and at different viewing distances. In particular, the importance of regions of the parietal cortex in the pathogenesis of neglect and in spatial attention and perception is discussed. Parietal cortex may control different distributions of attention across space by allocating specific attentional resources in near and far space while also showing attentional asymmetry across visual fields. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) as a technique offers the advantage of examining the direct behavioral effect of disruption of many of these areas with excellent temporal and spatial resolution. We discuss the use of TMS and the insights it may offer regarding the roles of these areas in neglect as well as normal visuospatial perception.

[1]  Igor Schindler,et al.  Citation for Published Item: Use Policy an Exploration of the Role of the Superior Temporal Gyrus in Visual Search and Spatial Perception Using Tms. Running Title: the Role of Stg Lesions in Neglect-like Tasks , 2022 .

[2]  Elizabeth Coulthard,et al.  Visual neglect after right posterior cerebral artery infarction , 2006, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[3]  Tracy L. Faber,et al.  Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.

[4]  J. Marshall,et al.  Spatial cognition: evidence from visual neglect , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  A. Cowey,et al.  Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  P. Benson,et al.  Towards a functional neuroanatomy of self processing: effects of faces and words. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[8]  G. Fink,et al.  Recovery from hemineglect: Differential neurobiological effects of optokinetic stimulation and alertness training , 2009, Cortex.

[9]  James L Patton,et al.  Mapping the Neglected Space , 2012, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[10]  A. Milner,et al.  Cerebral correlates of visual awareness , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Is there a critical lesion site for unilateral spatial neglect? A meta-analysis using activation likelihood estimation , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[12]  J. Marshall,et al.  Left neglect for near but not far space in man , 1991, Nature.

[13]  Andrea Serino,et al.  Fronto-parietal Areas Necessary for a Multisensory Representation of Peripersonal Space in Humans: An rTMS Study , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Geraint Rees,et al.  What can functional imaging reveal about the role of attention in visual awareness? , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  J. Driver,et al.  Perceptual awareness and its loss in unilateral neglect and extinction , 2001, Cognition.

[16]  Uwe Herwig,et al.  Using the International 10-20 EEG System for Positioning of Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2004, Brain Topography.

[17]  A. Sack Using non-invasive brain interference as a tool for mimicking spatial neglect in healthy volunteers. , 2010, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[18]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Temporal aspects of visual search studied by transcranial magnetic stimulation , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Richard Passingham,et al.  The Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Phonological Processing: An Investigation using rTMS , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[20]  H. Duffau,et al.  Direct Evidence for a Parietal-Frontal Pathway Subserving Spatial Awareness in Humans , 2005, Science.

[21]  S. Kastner,et al.  Shifting Attentional Priorities: Control of Spatial Attention through Hemispheric Competition , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  O. Tzeng,et al.  Far-space neglect in conjunction but not feature search following transcranial magnetic stimulation over right posterior parietal cortex. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  M. McCourt,et al.  Unilateral Visual Cueing and Asymmetric Line Geometry Share a Common Attentional Origin in the Modulation of Pseudoneglect , 2005, Cortex.

[24]  K. Zilles,et al.  Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  G. R. Fink,et al.  Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: A behavioural and fMRI study , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  M. Sale,et al.  Testing for Spatial Neglect with Line Bisection and Target Cancellation: Are Both Tasks Really Unrelated? , 2011, PloS one.

[27]  P. Bartolomeo,et al.  White matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neglect: Gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness , 2008, Cortex.

[28]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness , 1998, Nature.

[29]  Alan Cowey,et al.  Spatial neglect in near and far space investigated by repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  Beverly C. Butler,et al.  Visual search patterns in neglect: Comparison of peripersonal and extrapersonal space , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  A. Cowey,et al.  No abrupt change in visual hemineglect from near to far space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[32]  P. Medendorp,et al.  Visuospatial updating of reaching targets in near and far space , 2002, Neuroreport.

[33]  Silke M. Göbel,et al.  Parietal rTMS distorts the mental number line: Simulating ‘spatial’ neglect in healthy subjects , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  B. Fierro,et al.  Neglect-like effects induced by tDCS modulation of posterior parietal cortices in healthy subjects , 2011, Brain Stimulation.

[35]  R. Rafal,et al.  Neural fate of seen and unseen faces in visuospatial neglect: A combined event-related functional MRI and event-related potential study , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  M. Mesulam Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[37]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[38]  Lynn C. Robertson,et al.  Visual Search Performance in the Neglect Syndrome , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  T. Kuhlen,et al.  Horizontal and vertical pseudoneglect in peri- and extrapersonal space , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[40]  T. Vilis,et al.  Gaze-Centered Updating of Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Nicola Smania,et al.  Coding of far and near space during walking in neglect patients. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[42]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  M. Rushworth,et al.  The parietal cortex in visual search: a visuomotor hypothesis. , 2003, Supplements to Clinical neurophysiology.

[44]  Radek Ptak,et al.  Spatial neglect and the neural coding of attentional priority , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[45]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  A new neural framework for visuospatial processing , 2011, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  C. Guariglia,et al.  Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Alison R. Lane,et al.  Near and far space: Understanding the neural mechanisms of spatial attention , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[48]  G. Maira,et al.  A comment on impaired peri-nidal cerebrovascular reserve in seizure patients with brain arteriovenous malformations. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  J. J. Marottaa,et al.  Hemispatial neglect : its effects on visual perception and visually guided grasping , 2003 .

[50]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  'The anatomy underlying acute versus chronic spatial neglect' also depends on clinical tests. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Are action and perception in near and far space additive or interactive factors? , 2003, NeuroImage.

[52]  Neil G. Muggleton,et al.  TMS over right posterior parietal cortex induces neglect in a scene-based frame of reference , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  Pierluigi Zoccolotti,et al.  Functional evaluation of hemineglect by means of a semistructured scale: Personal extrapersonal differentiation , 1991 .

[54]  L. Tcheang,et al.  The Precuneus and Visuospatial Attention in Near and far Space: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study , 2014, Brain Stimulation.

[55]  S. Kataoka,et al.  Primary position upbeat nystagmus due to unilateral medial medullary infarction , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[56]  T Landis,et al.  Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect , 1998, Annals of neurology.

[57]  Lamme Vaf,et al.  Why visual attention and awareness are different , 2003 .

[58]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.