Is the right frontal cortex really crucial in the mentalizing network? A longitudinal study in patients with a slow-growing lesion

Assessing the subjective experience of others in terms of mental states, a brain function referred to as mentalizing, is achieved in the brain through a set of low-level perceptual and high-level inference-based processes. Because of its recurrent implication in fMRI studies, the right frontal cortex, especially in its inferolateral and dorsomesial parts, is posited to be a "core system" in the sustenance of these neurocognitive mechanisms. In this context, we reasoned that if the right frontal cortex is really crucial for mentalizing, its surgical resection, following diffuse low-grade glioma invasion, should induce irreversible impairments. To test this hypothesis, we designed a longitudinal experimental setup in which ten patients harboring a low-grade glioma in right frontal areas were assessed just before, immediately after and three months after a brain surgery. Two well-validated behavioral tasks, thought to evaluate both aspects of mentalizing, were administered. The results obtained provide evidence that widespread surgical excisions of the right prefrontal cortex do not induce a long-term worsening of both aspects of mentalizing, although some transitory effects are observed immediately after the surgery. They suggest also for the first time in the same sample of patients a possible double functional dissociation between low-level perceptual (posterior inferolateral prefrontal) and high-level inference-based (dorsomesial prefrontal) mentalizing processes. This overall finding challenges the traditional view according to which the right frontal cortex is an "essential cortical node" in the mentalizing network since it might be expected that massive surgical excisions of this brain area would have induced more definitive impairments.

[1]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Differential role of the orbital frontal lobe in emotional versus cognitive perspective-taking , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Koji Jimura,et al.  Activation of Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus during Response Inhibition across Response Modalities , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Dysfunction of the social brain in schizophrenia is modulated by intention type: an fMRI study. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[4]  Michael Erb,et al.  Task-specific activity and connectivity within the mentalizing network during emotion and intention mentalizing , 2011, NeuroImage.

[5]  S. Bressler Large-scale cortical networks and cognition , 1995, Brain Research Reviews.

[6]  Hugues Duffau,et al.  The “frontal syndrome” revisited: Lessons from electrostimulation mapping studies , 2012, Cortex.

[7]  J. Aharon-Peretz,et al.  Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  J. Decety,et al.  Abnormalities of brain function during a nonverbal theory of mind task in schizophrenia , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  Hugues Duffau,et al.  Does post-lesional subcortical plasticity exist in the human brain? , 2009, Neuroscience Research.

[10]  Marco Catani,et al.  From hodology to function. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The Busy Social Brain , 2013, Psychological science.

[12]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  The Development of Human Functional Brain Networks , 2010, Neuron.

[13]  C. Keysers,et al.  Integrating simulation and theory of mind: from self to social cognition , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Donna B. Pincus,et al.  Distinguishing Lies from Jokes: Theory of Mind Deficits and Discourse Interpretation in Right Hemisphere Brain-Damaged Patients , 1998, Brain and Language.

[15]  L. Douw,et al.  Cognitive and radiological effects of radiotherapy in patients with low-grade glioma: long-term follow-up , 2009, The Lancet Neurology.

[16]  G. Coricelli Two-levels of mental states attribution: from automaticity to voluntariness , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[17]  D. Poeppel,et al.  The cortical organization of speech processing , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  H. Duffau,et al.  Assessment of verbal working memory before and after surgery for low-grade glioma , 2007, Journal of Neuro-Oncology.

[19]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[21]  Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  H. Duffau Lessons from brain mapping in surgery for low-grade glioma: insights into associations between tumour and brain plasticity , 2005, The Lancet Neurology.

[23]  Yaniv Assaf,et al.  Learning in the Fast Lane: New Insights into Neuroplasticity , 2012, Neuron.

[24]  U. Frith,et al.  The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on 'Theory of Mind' and cognition. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  Andrew L. Mackinnon,et al.  Estimation de l’intelligence prémorbide chez les francophones , 2005 .

[26]  Laurent Capelle,et al.  Continuous growth of mean tumor diameter in a subset of grade II gliomas , 2003, Annals of neurology.

[27]  R. Knight,et al.  Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Fuster The cognit: a network model of cortical representation. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[29]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  An integrative model of the neural systems supporting the comprehension of observed emotional behavior , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  M. Catani,et al.  The rises and falls of disconnection syndromes. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  Biyu J. He,et al.  Breakdown of Functional Connectivity in Frontoparietal Networks Underlies Behavioral Deficits in Spatial Neglect , 2007, Neuron.

[32]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Solving the paradox of the equipotential and modular brain: A neurocomputational model of stroke vs. slow-growing glioma , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[33]  D Le Bihan,et al.  Role of the healthy hemisphere in recovery after resection of the supplementary motor area , 2004, Neurology.

[34]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[35]  M. Just,et al.  From the Selectedworks of Marcel Adam Just the Organization of Thinking: What Functional Brain Imaging Reveals about the Neuroarchitecture of Complex Cognition , 2022 .

[36]  S. Shamay-Tsoory The Neural Bases for Empathy , 2011, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[37]  W. van den Bos,et al.  Toward an integrative account of social cognition: marrying theory of mind and interactionism to study the interplay of Type 1 and Type 2 processes , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[38]  Henrik Walter,et al.  Intentional Minds: A Philosophical Analysis of Intention Tested through fMRI Experiments Involving People with Schizophrenia, People with Autism, and Healthy Individuals , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[39]  V. Gallese Before and below ‘theory of mind’: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[40]  Hans-Otto Karnath,et al.  Do brain tumours allow valid conclusions on the localisation of human brain functions? – Objections , 2011, Cortex.

[41]  Stefano F. Cappa,et al.  The Neural Bases of Social Intention Understanding: The Role of Interaction Goals , 2012, PloS one.

[42]  H. Duffau,et al.  Combination of neoadjuvant chemotherapy followed by surgical resection as a new strategy for WHO grade II gliomas: a study of cognitive status and quality of life , 2011, Journal of Neuro-Oncology.

[43]  Andrew Simmons,et al.  Beyond cortical localization in clinico-anatomical correlation , 2012, Cortex.

[44]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[45]  J Aharon-Peretz,et al.  Impaired “Affective Theory of Mind” Is Associated with Right Ventromedial Prefrontal Damage , 2005, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[46]  R. Mar The neural bases of social cognition and story comprehension. , 2011, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  Roel M. Willems,et al.  Complementary Systems for Understanding Action Intentions , 2008, Current Biology.

[48]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  Sophia Vinogradov,et al.  Theory of Mind Skills Are Related to Gray Matter Volume in the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Schizophrenia , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[51]  F. Happé,et al.  Acquired mind-blindness following frontal lobe surgery? A single case study of impaired ‘theory of mind’ in a patient treated with stereotactic anterior capsulotomy , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  Nikos Paragios,et al.  Joint Tumor Segmentation and Dense Deformable Registration of Brain MR Images , 2012, MICCAI.

[53]  Reginald B. Adams,et al.  Cross-cultural Reading the Mind in the Eyes: An fMRI Investigation , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Spatial Normalization of Brain Images with Focal Lesions Using Cost Function Masking , 2001, NeuroImage.

[55]  Hugues Duffau,et al.  Awake surgery for WHO Grade II gliomas within "noneloquent" areas in the left dominant hemisphere: toward a "supratotal" resection. Clinical article. , 2011, Journal of neurosurgery.

[56]  John Duncan,et al.  The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[57]  Laurent Capelle,et al.  Preferential brain locations of low‐grade gliomas , 2004, Cancer.

[58]  Heidi E Kirsch,et al.  Resting functional connectivity in patients with brain tumors in eloquent areas , 2011, Annals of neurology.

[59]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: a review of core processes. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[60]  M J Brammer,et al.  Exploring the social brain in schizophrenia: left prefrontal underactivation during mental state attribution. , 2000, The American journal of psychiatry.

[61]  S. Bressler,et al.  Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[62]  Charles D. Smith,et al.  Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[63]  John Suckling,et al.  Shared Neural Circuits for Mentalizing about the Self and Others , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[65]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  What’s so special about Asperger Syndrome? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[67]  H. Damasio,et al.  Neuropsychological impairments associated with lesions caused by tumor or stroke. , 1990, Archives of neurology.

[68]  H. Duffau,et al.  Direct Evidence for a Parietal-Frontal Pathway Subserving Spatial Awareness in Humans , 2005, Science.

[69]  Marsel Mesulam,et al.  Defining Neurocognitive Networks in the BOLD New World of Computed Connectivity , 2009, Neuron.

[70]  O. Sporns,et al.  The economy of brain network organization , 2012, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[71]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[72]  A. Zangen,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs theory of mind learning , 2012, European Psychiatry.

[73]  V. Gallese,et al.  What is so special about embodied simulation? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[74]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[75]  Y. Tabei,et al.  Damage to the right superior longitudinal fasciculus in the inferior parietal lobe plays a role in spatial neglect , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[76]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Mirror Neuron and Theory of Mind Mechanisms Involved in Face-to-Face Interactions: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Approach to Empathy , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[77]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Identifying the What, Why, and How of an Observed Action: An fMRI Study of Mentalizing and Mechanizing during Action Observation , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[78]  Elsa D. Angelini,et al.  Evidence for potentials and limitations of brain plasticity using an atlas of functional resectability of WHO grade II gliomas: Towards a “minimal common brain” , 2011, NeuroImage.

[79]  R. Morris,et al.  Differential Effects of Lesions of the Amygdala and Prefrontal Cortex on Recognizing Facial Expressions of Complex Emotions , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[81]  A. Bailey,et al.  Are there theory of mind regions in the brain? A review of the neuroimaging literature , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[82]  Rebecca Elliott,et al.  Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task , 2006, NeuroImage.

[83]  S. Shamay-Tsoory,et al.  Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[84]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[85]  H. Duffau,et al.  White matter functional connectivity as an additional landmark for dominant temporal lobectomy , 2008, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry.

[86]  Francesca Baglio,et al.  Effects of aging on mindreading ability through the eyes: An fMRI study , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[87]  Bregtje Gunther Moor,et al.  Neurodevelopmental changes of reading the mind in the eyes. , 2012, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[88]  M. Sabbagh,et al.  Understanding orbitofrontal contributions to theory-of-mind reasoning: Implications for autism , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[89]  Olaf Sporns,et al.  Can structure predict function in the human brain? , 2010, NeuroImage.

[90]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[91]  H. Duffau,et al.  Speaking without Broca's area after tumor resection , 2009, Neurocase.

[92]  Bruno Dubois,et al.  A novel approach to clinical–radiological correlations: Anatomo-Clinical Overlapping Maps (AnaCOM): Method and validation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[93]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .

[94]  H. Walter,et al.  The intentional network: How the brain reads varieties of intentions , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[95]  H. Duffau,et al.  Cognition and resective surgery for diffuse infiltrative glioma: an overview , 2012, Journal of Neuro-Oncology.

[96]  Dennis A. Nowak,et al.  Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: A TMS study , 2010, Cortex.

[97]  Y. Sarfati,et al.  Attribution of intentions to others in people with schizophrenia: a non-verbal exploration with comic strips , 1997, Schizophrenia Research.

[98]  Pélagie M. Beeson,et al.  Cost function masking during normalization of brains with focal lesions: Still a necessity? , 2010, NeuroImage.

[99]  D. Stuss,et al.  The frontal lobes are necessary for 'theory of mind'. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[100]  Christine Passerieux,et al.  Anatomical and temporal architecture of theory of mind: A MEG insight into the early stages , 2011, NeuroImage.

[101]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[102]  M. Desmurget,et al.  Contrasting acute and slow-growing lesions: a new door to brain plasticity. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[103]  Milena Rabovsky,et al.  The time course of semantic richness effects in visual word recognition , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[104]  Hugues Duffau,et al.  Brain Hodotopy: From Esoteric Concept to Practical Surgical Applications , 2011, Neurosurgery.

[105]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.