Response selection in prosaccades, antisaccades, and other volitional saccades

Saccades made to the opposite side of a visual stimulus (antisaccades) and to central cues (simple volitional saccades) both require active response selection but whether the mechanisms of response selection differ between these tasks is unclear. Response selection can be assessed by increasing the number of response alternatives: this leads to increased reaction times when response selection is more demanding. We compared the reaction times of prosaccades, antisaccades, saccades cued by a central arrow, and saccades cued by a central number, in blocks of either two or six possible responses. In the two-response blocks, reaction times were fastest for prosaccades and antisaccades, and slowest for arrow-cued and number-cued saccades. Increasing response alternatives from two to six caused a paradoxical reduction in reaction times of prosaccades, had no effect on arrow-cued saccades, and led to a large increase in reaction times of number-cued saccades. For antisaccade reaction times, the effect of increasing response alternatives was intermediate, greater than that for arrow-cued saccades but less than that for number-cued saccades. We suggest that this pattern of results may reflect two components of saccadic processing: (a) response triggering, which is more rapid with a peripheral stimulus as in the prosaccade and antisaccade tasks and (b) response selection, which is more demanding for the antisaccade and number-cued saccade tasks, and more automatic when there is direct stimulus–response mapping as with prosaccades, or over-learned symbols as with arrow-cued saccades.

[1]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Behavioral properties of saccades generated as a choice response , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  Lawrence H Snyder,et al.  The Responses of Visual Neurons in the Frontal Eye Field Are Biased for Saccades , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  R. Remington Attention and saccadic eye movements. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  Benedikt Reuter,et al.  Impaired action control in schizophrenia: The role of volitional saccade initiation , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  B. Reuter,et al.  Evidence for a deficit in volitional action generation in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[7]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Why are antisaccades slower than prosaccades? A novel finding using a new paradigm , 2003, Neuroreport.

[8]  Bonnie M. Lawrence,et al.  Saccades and reaches, behaving differently , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  H. Hughes,et al.  Effect of uncertainty on the time course for selection of verbal name codes , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Benedikt Reuter,et al.  Distinct Neural Correlates for Volitional Generation and Inhibition of Saccades , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  J Ashe,et al.  Choice and stimulus-response compatibility affect duration of response selection. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[12]  P. Fitts,et al.  S-R compatibility: spatial characteristics of stimulus and response codes. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[14]  S. Hutton,et al.  The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[15]  Stephen J. Heinen,et al.  Properties of saccades generated as a choice response , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  J. Ashe,et al.  The Effect of Stimulus–Response Compatibility on Cortical Motor Activation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[17]  Jun Zhang,et al.  Flexible filaments in a flowing soap film as a model for one-dimensional flags in a two-dimensional wind , 2000, Nature.

[18]  Kestutis Kveraga,et al.  Effects of stimulus-response uncertainty on saccades to near-threshold targets , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Bonnie M. Lawrence,et al.  Manipulations of the relationship between response alternatives and exogenous saccade latencies , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  André Vandierendonck,et al.  Input monitoring and response selection as components of executive control in pro-saccades and anti-saccades , 2007, Psychological research.

[21]  M Eimer,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  M. Masson Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. , 2003, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[23]  Takashi R Sato,et al.  Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.

[24]  Robin Walker,et al.  Control of voluntary and reflexive saccades , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  Benedikt Reuter,et al.  Effects of switching between leftward and rightward pro- and antisaccades , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[26]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Locus of the effect of the number of alternative responses : Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential , 1998 .

[27]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[28]  W. E. Hick Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.

[29]  Kestutis Kveraga,et al.  Effects of Directional Uncertainty on Visually-Guided Joystick Pointing , 2005, Perceptual and motor skills.

[30]  B. Reuter,et al.  Impaired volitional saccade control: first evidence for a new candidate endophenotype in obsessive–compulsive disorder , 2013, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[31]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Saccades operate in violation of Hick’s law , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  A. Kingstone,et al.  A new look at aging and performance in the antisaccade task: The impact of response selection , 2009 .

[33]  G. Kuhn,et al.  Look away! Eyes and arrows engage oculomotor responses automatically , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[34]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  Neuronal switching of sensorimotor transformations for antisaccades , 2000, Nature.

[35]  Matti S. Hämäläinen,et al.  Where left becomes right: A magnetoencephalographic study of sensorimotor transformation for antisaccades , 2007, NeuroImage.

[36]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[37]  Christopher Kennard,et al.  Differential cortical activation during voluntary and reflexive saccades in man , 2003, NeuroImage.

[38]  C. Chubb,et al.  Exploring attention-based explanations for some violations of Hick’s law for aimed movements , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Charles Chubb,et al.  Visually guided, aimed movements can be unaffected by stimulus–response uncertainty , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Lawrence H Snyder,et al.  An anti-Hick's effect in monkey and human saccade reaction times. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[41]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  Kyoung-Min Lee,et al.  Effect of the number of response alternatives on brain activity in response selection , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[43]  Alex R. Wade,et al.  Differential correlation of frontal and parietal activity with the number of alternatives for cued choice saccades , 2006, NeuroImage.

[44]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[45]  Bonnie M. Lawrence,et al.  An anti-Hick’s effect for exogenous, but not endogenous, saccadic eye movements , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[46]  W H Teichner,et al.  Laws of visual choice reaction time. , 1974, Psychological review.