Control variables and proprioceptive feedback in fast single-joint movement.

Sensorimotor mechanisms were studied on the basis of kinematic and electromyographic data as well as the static torque developed by the muscles as a function of joint angle. The latter relationship is known as the torque/angle characteristic. Fast single-joint movement may result from a shift in this characteristic and a change in its slope. Such movements were studied at the wrist in 9 normal and 1 deafferented subject. After training to flex the wrist to a target, subjects repeated the same movements but in random test trials movements were opposed by the load generated by linear position feedback to a torque motor. At the end of the loaded trials, the load was suddenly removed. In the second experiment, subjects made wrist movements to the target that were opposed by the load and, on random test trials, the movements were not loaded. In these test trials, the wrist arrived in a static position outside the target zone. In both experiments, subjects were instructed not to correct errors. The final torque/angle characteristics specified in the movements were reconstructed on the basis of the static wrist positions and torques before and after unloading. Normal subjects made movements by shifting the position of the torque/angle characteristic and by increasing its slope. If subjects indeed maintained the same pattern of control variables (descending commands), the same final position of the characteristic would be reproduced from trial to trial regardless of load perturbations. This assumption of equifinality was tested by comparing the final position of the wrist in nonloaded movements with that after removal of the load in loaded movements. Equifinality was observed in normal subjects. Movements in the deafferented subject were also associated with a shift of the torque/angle characteristic and a change in its slope. However, she was unable to consistently reproduce its final position. In spite of muscle coactivation, her maximal stiffness was lower than in normal subjects. In the absence of vision, the subject made movements with the load by increasing the slope of the characteristic instead of by shifting its position far enough. Load perturbation affected her final wrist position (inequifinality), which may reflect the presence of a significant hysteresis of the characteristic as a result of the absence of stretch reflexes. The deficits following deafferentation presumably result from the destruction of biomechanical and sensorimotor mechanisms including the ability of control variables to specify the positional frame of reference for afferent and descending systems.

[1]  P. Matthews The dependence of tension upon extension in the stretch reflex of the soleus muscle of the decerebrate cat , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  P. Matthews MUSCLE SPINDLES AND THEIR MOTOR CONTROL. , 1964, Physiological reviews.

[3]  J. Foley The co-ordination and regulation of movements , 1968 .

[4]  P. Rack,et al.  The effects of length and stimulus rate on tension in the isometric cat soleus muscle , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  R. Granit The basis of motor control , 1970 .

[6]  A. G. Feldman,et al.  The influence of different descending systems on the tonic stretch reflex in the cat. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[7]  M. Hallett,et al.  EMG analysis of stereotyped voluntary movements in man. , 1975, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[8]  J. Houk An assessment of stretch reflex function. , 1976, Progress in brain research.

[9]  J. Houk,et al.  Improvement in linearity and regulation of stiffness that results from actions of stretch reflex. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  J. Houk,et al.  Regulatory actions of human stretch reflex. , 1976, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  A. G. Feldman Superposition of motor programs—II. Rapid forearm flexion in man , 1980, Neuroscience.

[12]  S. Andreassen,et al.  Regulation of soleus muscle stiffness in premammillary cats: intrinsic and reflex components. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  J A Kelso,et al.  Analysis of "invariant characteristics" in the motor control of down's syndrome and normal subjects. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[14]  B L Day,et al.  Accurate repositioning of the human thumb against unpredictable dynamic loads is dependent upon peripheral feed‐back , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  L. Cohen,et al.  Changes in the N1-P1 component of the somatosensory cortical evoked response in patients with partial limb amputation. , 1984, Electromyography and clinical neurophysiology.

[16]  M. Cynader,et al.  Somatosensory cortical map changes following digit amputation in adult monkeys , 1984, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[17]  A. G. Feldman Once More on the Equilibrium-Point Hypothesis (λ Model) for Motor Control , 1986 .

[18]  O. I. Fukson,et al.  Adaptability of innate motor patterns and motor control mechanisms , 1986, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[19]  R. Lee,et al.  Modification of Motor Output to Compensate for Unanticipated Load Conditions During Rapid Voluntary Movements , 1986, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[20]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Rapid elbow flexion in the absence of proprioceptive and cutaneous feedback. , 1987, Human neurobiology.

[21]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Compliance of single joints: elastic and plastic characteristics. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Jeannerod The neural and behavioural organization of goal-directed movements , 1990, Psychological Medicine.

[23]  G. Gottlieb,et al.  Strategies for the control of voluntary movements with one mechanical degree of freedom , 1989, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  T. Sinkjaer,et al.  Regulation of wrist stiffness by the stretch reflex. , 1989, Journal of biomechanics.

[25]  T. Nichols The organization of heterogenic reflexes among muscles crossing the ankle joint in the decerebrate cat. , 1989, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  R. N. Lemon,et al.  Non-invasive brain stimulation reveals reorganised cortical outputs in amputees , 1990, Neuroscience Letters.

[27]  Yves Lamarre,et al.  Anticipatory postural adjustment in the absence of normal peripheral feedback , 1990, Brain Research.

[28]  D. Rasmusson,et al.  Acute effects of total or partial digit denervation on raccoon somatosensory cortex. , 1990, Somatosensory & motor research.

[29]  C Ghez,et al.  Roles of proprioceptive input in the programming of arm trajectories. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[30]  S. Bandinelli,et al.  Motor reorganization after upper limb amputation in man. A study with focal magnetic stimulation. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[31]  J. Kaas Plasticity of sensory and motor maps in adult mammals. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  J. Paillard Motor and representational framing of space , 1991 .

[33]  D. Burke,et al.  Does the nervous system depend on kinesthetic information to control natural limb movements , 1992 .

[34]  Anatol G. Feldman,et al.  Fundamentals of motor control, kinesthesia and spinal neurons: In search of a theory , 1992 .

[35]  U. Windhorst,et al.  Dynamic behaviour of α-motoneurons subjected to recurrent inhibition and reflex feedback via muscle spindles , 1992, Neuroscience.

[36]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[37]  S. Hahn Motor Reorganization After Upper Limb Amputation in Man , 1992 .

[38]  D. Louw,et al.  The immediate effects of peripheral denervation on inhibitory mechanisms in the somatosensory thalamus. , 1993, Somatosensory & motor research.

[39]  M. Latash Control of human movement , 1993 .