Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment.

When 16 children with SLI (mean age = 6;2) and 16 normally developing age-mates named age-appropriate objects, the SLI cohort made more naming errors. For both cohorts, semantic misnaming and indeterminate responses were the predominant error types. The contribution of limited semantic representation to these naming errors was explored. Each participant drew and defined each item from his or her semantic and indeterminate error pools and each item from his or her correctly named pool. When compared, the drawings and definitions of items from the error pools were poorer, suggesting limited semantic knowledge. The profiles of information included in definitions of items from the correct pool and the error pools were highly similar, suggesting that representations associated with misnaming differed quanlitatively, but not qualitatively, from those associated with correct naming. Eleven members of the SLI cohort also participated in a forced-choice recognition task. Performance was significantly lower on erroneous targets than on correctly named targets. When performance was compared across all three post-naming tasks (drawing, defining, recognition), the participants evinced sparse semantic knowledge for roughly half of all semantic misnaming and roughly one third of all indeterminate responses. In additional cases, representational gaps were evident. This study demonstrates that the degree of knowledge represented in the child's semantic lexicon makes words more or less vulnerable to retrieval failure and that limited semantic knowledge contributes to the frequent naming errors of children with SLI.

[1]  J. Mandler Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Infancy , 2000 .

[2]  A. Baddeley,et al.  When long-term learning depends on short-term storage , 1988 .

[3]  G. Miller,et al.  Linguistic theory and psychological reality , 1982 .

[4]  M L Rice,et al.  Specific-language-impaired children's quick incidental learning of words: the effect of a pause. , 1992, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[5]  C. Dollaghan,et al.  Spoken word recognition in children with and without specific language impairment , 1998, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[6]  P. Tallal,et al.  Rapid Automatized Naming and gesture by normal and language-impaired children , 1992, Brain and Language.

[7]  T. Campbell,et al.  Reducing bias in language assessment: processing-dependent measures. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[8]  Mutsumi Imai,et al.  Children's Theories of Word Meaning: The Role of Shape Similarity in Early Acquisition , 1994 .

[9]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study , 1989 .

[10]  Lynne E. Hewitt,et al.  Children with Specific Language Impairment , 2002 .

[11]  R. Cheloha,et al.  The of a Development , 2004 .

[12]  Byers Vw,et al.  Guidelines for identification audiometry. , 1975, ASHA.

[13]  K. McGregor The nature of word-finding errors of preschoolers with and without word-finding deficits. , 1997, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[14]  J B Tomblin,et al.  Clinical decision making: describing the decision rules of practicing speech-language pathologists. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[15]  K. McGregor,et al.  On the relation between mental representation and naming in a child with specific language impairment , 2002, Clinical linguistics & phonetics.

[16]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Concepts and Their Surface Representations , 1984 .

[17]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[18]  S. Waxman,et al.  Object naming at multiple hierarchical levels: a comparison of preschoolers with and without word-finding deficits , 1998, Journal of Child Language.

[19]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? ☆ , 1990 .

[20]  David F. Bjorklund,et al.  How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children's memory: An interpretive review , 1987 .

[21]  R. Watkins,et al.  Measuring children's lexical diversity: differentiating typical and impaired language learners. , 1995, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[22]  J. Montgomery Examination of phonological working memory in specifically language-impaired children , 1995, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[23]  H. Storkel,et al.  Learning new words: phonotactic probability in language development. , 2001, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[24]  Dare A. Baldwin,et al.  Clarifying the role of shape in children's taxonomic assumption. , 1992, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  M. A. Lindberg Is knowledge base development a necessary and sufficient condition for memory development? , 1980, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[26]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[27]  L. Leonard,et al.  Facilitating word-finding skills of language-impaired children. , 1989, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[28]  Irving Lorge,et al.  Columbia mental maturity scale , 1954 .

[29]  J. M. Anglin,et al.  From Reference to Meaning. , 1978 .

[30]  M. Chi,et al.  Network representation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. , 1983 .

[31]  R. Kail,et al.  Picture naming in language-impaired children. , 1983, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[32]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Mechanisms for accessing lexical representations for output: Evidence from a category-specific semantic deficit , 1991, Brain and Language.

[33]  L. Gershkoff-Stowe The Course of Children's Naming Errors in Early Word Learning , 2001 .

[34]  J B Tomblin,et al.  Nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[35]  K. Nelson,et al.  Making sense : the acquisition of shared meaning , 1985 .

[36]  Mabel L. Rice,et al.  Specific Language Impairments in Children. , 1994 .

[37]  C. Dollaghan,et al.  Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. , 1987, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[38]  E. Bjork,et al.  The development of word retrieval abilities in the second year and its relation to early vocabulary growth. , 2000, Child development.

[39]  C. Roth Factors affecting developmental changes in the speed of processing. , 1983, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[40]  S. Wright Teaching word-finding strategies to severely language-impaired children. , 1993, European journal of disorders of communication : the journal of the College of Speech and Language Therapists, London.

[41]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Curvilinear Trend in Naming Errors as a Function of Early Vocabulary Growth , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[42]  L B Leonard,et al.  Word-finding abilities in language-impaired children. , 1986, ASHA monographs.

[43]  S. Carey The child as word learner , 1978 .

[44]  J. Edwards,et al.  Why do children with specific language impairment name pictures more slowly than their peers? , 1996, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[45]  P. Tallal,et al.  Different origin of auditory and phonological processing problems in children with language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[46]  S. Luthar,et al.  The construct of resilience: a critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. , 2000, Child development.

[47]  Frank Wijnen Incidental word and sound errors in young speakers , 1991 .

[48]  K. McGregor,et al.  Semantic representation and naming in young children. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[49]  J Edwards,et al.  Naming errors of children with specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[50]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. , 1992 .

[51]  M L Rice,et al.  Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. , 1994, Journal of speech and hearing research.

[52]  M. Dennis,et al.  Development of word-finding , 1986, Brain and Language.

[53]  M. Rice,et al.  Fast mapping word-learning abilities of language-delayed preschoolers. , 1990, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[54]  R. Kail,et al.  Word-Finding Abilities in Language-Impaired Children: ASHA Monographs Number 25. , 1986 .

[55]  C Donlan,et al.  Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. , 1996, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[56]  A. Vermeer Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input , 2001, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[57]  Rosalind Horowitz,et al.  Comprehending Oral and Written Language. , 1987 .

[58]  Julia L. Evans,et al.  An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. , 1999, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[59]  T. Campbell,et al.  Nonword repetition and child language impairment. , 1998, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.