Activity in older adults: cause or consequence of cognitive functioning? A longitudinal study on everyday activities and cognitive performance in older adults.

The impact of three types of everyday activities (i.e., social, experiential, and developmental) on four cognitive functions (i.e., immediate recall, learning, fluid intelligence, and information-processing speed) and one global indicator of cognitive functioning (Mini-Mental State Exam score) over a period of 6 years was studied in a large 55--85 year-old population-based sample (N = 2,076). A cross-lagged regression model with latent variables was applied to each combination of 1 cognitive function and 1 type of activity, resulting in 15 (3 x 5) different models. None of the activities were found to enhance cognitive functioning 6 years later when controlling for age, gender, level of education, and health, as well as for unknown confounding variables. Conversely, one cognitive function (i.e., information-processing speed) appeared to affect developmental activity. It is suggested that no specific activity, but rather socioeconomic status to which activities are closely connected, contributes to maintenance of cognitive functions.

[1]  R. Dixon,et al.  On the problem of detecting effects of lifestyle on cognitive change in adulthood : Reply to Pushkar et al. (1999) , 1999 .

[2]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  The role of psychosocial context, age, and intelligence in memory performance of older men. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[3]  R. Dixon,et al.  Multidisciplinary propositions on the development of intelligence during the adulthood and old age , 1986 .

[4]  Carmi Schooler,et al.  The Reciprocal Effects of the Substantive Complexity of Work and Intellectual Flexibility: A Longitudinal Assessment , 1978, American Journal of Sociology.

[5]  B. Penninx,et al.  Do disease specific characteristics add to the explanation of mobility limitations in patients with different chronic diseases? A study in The Netherlands. , 1997, Journal of epidemiology and community health.

[6]  C. Brayne,et al.  Cognitive decline in an elderly population – a two wave study of change , 1995, Psychological Medicine.

[7]  Analyzing Change over Time Using LISREL , 1994 .

[8]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[9]  R. Sprott Age, learning ability, and intelligence , 1980 .

[10]  P. Baltes The aging mind: potential and limits. , 1993, The Gerontologist.

[11]  D. Wegener,et al.  The problem of equivalent models in applications of covariance structure analysis. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[12]  K. Schaie,et al.  Complexity of life style and maintenance of intellectual abilities. , 1980 .

[13]  R. Dixon,et al.  Use it or lose it: engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[14]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Cognitive functioning of older people in relation to social and personality variables. , 1986, Psychology and aging.

[15]  D. Hultsch,et al.  Age differences in cognitive performance in later life: relationships to self-reported health and activity life style. , 1993, Journal of gerontology.

[16]  A. Rey L'examen clinique in psychologie , 1964 .

[17]  T. Tombaugh,et al.  The Mini‐Mental State Examination: A Comprehensive Review , 1992, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[18]  J. Groothoff,et al.  Socio-economic differences in health risk behavior in adolescence: do they exist? , 1998, Social science & medicine.

[19]  K. Warner Schaie,et al.  Cognitive Functioning and Social Structure over the Life Course , 1987 .

[20]  Robert T. Golembiewski,et al.  Measuring Change and Persistence in Human Affairs: Types of Change Generated by OD Designs , 1976 .

[21]  P. Baltes,et al.  Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[22]  M. Kohn,et al.  Continuity of Learning-Generalization: The Effect of Job on Men's Intellective Process in the United States and Poland , 1985, American Journal of Sociology.

[23]  L. Hayduk Structural equation modeling with LISREL , 1987 .

[24]  Richard B. Davies,et al.  Analyzing social and political change : a casebook of methods , 1994 .

[25]  M. Lezak Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. , 1995 .

[26]  D. Commenges,et al.  Social and Leisure Activities and Risk of Dementia: A Prospective Longitudinal Study , 1995, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[27]  Wan,et al.  LISREL analyses of interaction effects in multiple regression , 1996 .

[28]  J. Jaccard,et al.  LISREL Approaches to Interaction Effects in Multiple Regression , 1998 .

[29]  M. Frese,et al.  Longitudinal studies in organizational stress research: a review of the literature with reference to methodological issues. , 1996, Journal of occupational health psychology.

[30]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  The course and causes of cognitive change with advancing age , 1991 .

[31]  H. Lentzner Autonomy and well-being in the aging population: Concepts and design of the longitudinal aging study Amsterdam: Edited by D. J. H. Deeg, C. P. M. Knipscheer and W. van Tilburg. Institute of Gerontology, The Netherlands, 1993. 303 pp. U.S. $ , 1995 .

[32]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Models of intelligence in late life: comment on Hultsch et al. (1999). , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[33]  R. P. McDonald,et al.  Structural Equations with Latent Variables , 1989 .

[34]  T. Arbuckle,et al.  Structural equation model of intellectual change and continuity and predictors of intelligence in older men. , 1995, Psychology and Aging.

[35]  M. Perlmutter,et al.  Relationships between self-reported physical and mental health and intelligence performance across adulthood. , 1990, Journal of gerontology.

[36]  P. Baltes,et al.  Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. , 1990 .

[37]  T. Louis,et al.  Chronic conditions that lead to functional limitation in the elderly. , 1994, Journal of gerontology.

[38]  A. Mackinnon,et al.  A prospective study of cognitive function in the elderly , 1997, Psychological Medicine.

[39]  C. Schooler,et al.  The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[40]  Pearl A. Dykstra,et al.  Living arrangements and social networks of older adults , 1996 .

[41]  T. Salthouse The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. , 1996, Psychological review.

[42]  Leslie A. Hayduk Structural equation modeling with LISREL: essentials and advances , 1987 .

[43]  S. Porter,et al.  Ageing in Society: An Introduction to Social Gerontology , 1994 .

[44]  C. Jonker,et al.  Social participation and cognitive functioning in older adults , 1995 .