‘They hear “Africa” and they think that there can’t be any good services’ – perceived context in cross-national learning: a qualitative study of the barriers to Reverse Innovation

BackgroundCountry-of-origin of a product can negatively influence its rating, particularly if the product is from a low-income country. It follows that how non-traditional sources of innovation, such as low-income countries, are perceived is likely to be an important part of a diffusion process, particularly given the strong social and cognitive boundaries associated with the healthcare professions.MethodsBetween September and December 2014, we conducted eleven in-depth face-to-face or telephone interviews with key informants from innovation, health and social policy circles, experts in international comparative policy research and leaders in Reverse Innovation in the United States. Interviews were open-ended with guiding probes into the barriers and enablers to Reverse Innovation in the US context, specifically also to understand whether, in their experience translating or attempting to translate innovations from low-income contexts into the US, the source of the innovation matters in the adopter context. Interviews were recorded, transcribed and analyzed thematically using the process of constant comparison.ResultsOur findings show that innovations from low-income countries tend to be discounted early on because of prior assumptions about the potential for these contexts to offer solutions to healthcare problems in the US. Judgments are made about the similarity of low-income contexts with the US, even though this is based oftentimes on flimsy perceptions only. Mixing levels of analysis, local and national, leads to country-level stereotyping and missed opportunities to learn from low-income countries.ConclusionsOur research highlights that prior expectations, invoked by the Low-income country cue, are interfering with a transparent and objective learning process. There may be merit in adopting some techniques from the cognitive psychology and marketing literatures to understand better the relative importance of source in healthcare research and innovation diffusion. Counter-stereotyping techniques and decision-making tools may be useful to help decision-makers evaluate the generalizability of research findings objectively and transparently. We suggest that those interested in Reverse Innovation should reflect carefully on the value of disclosing the source of the innovation that is being proposed, if doing so is likely to invoke negative stereotypes.

[1]  Damon Centola Damon Centola Behavior An Experimental Study of Homophily in the Adoption of Health , 2011 .

[2]  Stuart E. Madnick,et al.  Homophily and the Speed of Social Mobilization: The Effect of Acquired and Ascribed Traits , 2014, PloS one.

[3]  A. Haines,et al.  Achieving child survival goals: potential contribution of community health workers , 2007, The Lancet.

[4]  A. Strauss,et al.  The Discovery of Grounded Theory , 1967 .

[5]  M. McPherson,et al.  Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks , 2001 .

[6]  E. Rogers,et al.  Diffusion of innovations , 1964, Encyclopedia of Sport Management.

[7]  A. Rosen Destigmatizing day-to-day practices: what developed countries can learn from developing countries. , 2006, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[8]  D. Mascia,et al.  Unfolding similarity in interphysician networks: the impact of institutional and professional homophily , 2015, BMC Health Services Research.

[9]  Patricia Dandonoli Open innovation as a new paradigm for global collaborations in health , 2013, Globalization and Health.

[10]  M. Petticrew,et al.  Trading quality for relevance: non-health decision-makers’ use of evidence on the social determinants of health , 2015, BMJ Open.

[11]  J. Zwanziger,et al.  The relationship between safety net activities and hospital financial performance , 2010, BMC health services research.

[12]  Lord Nigel Crisp Turning the world upside down , 2010 .

[13]  WPA and disaster response: new policies and actions. , 2006, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[14]  S. Syed,et al.  Reverse innovation in global health systems: towards global innovation flow , 2013, Globalization and Health.

[15]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site , 2002 .

[16]  J. Ritchie,et al.  Qualitative Research Practice: A Guide for Social Science Students and Researchers , 2013 .

[17]  유창조 Naturalistic Inquiry , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[18]  Damon Centola An Experimental Study of Homophily in the Adoption of Health Behavior , 2011, Science.

[19]  J. Macinko,et al.  Does a research article's country of origin affect perception of its quality and relevance? A national trial of US public health researchers , 2015, BMJ Open.

[20]  R. Peterson,et al.  A Meta-Analysis of Country-of-Origin Effects , 1995 .

[21]  Durairaj Maheswaran,et al.  Cultural Variations in Country of Origin Effects , 2000 .

[22]  M. Banaji,et al.  Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. , 1995, Psychological review.

[23]  D. Maheswaran,et al.  Determinants of Country-of-Origin Evaluations , 2000 .

[24]  G. Robert,et al.  Diffusion of innovations in service organizations: systematic review and recommendations. , 2004, The Milbank quarterly.

[25]  Matthew B. Miles,et al.  Qualitative Data Analysis: An Expanded Sourcebook , 1994 .

[26]  R. Wyer,et al.  Effects of stereotypes on decision making and information-processing strategies. , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  V. Patel,et al.  Learning from low income countries: mental health , 2004, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[28]  W. Bilkey,et al.  Country-of-Origin Effects on Product Evaluations , 1982 .

[29]  J. DePasse,et al.  A model for ‘reverse innovation’ in health care , 2013, Globalization and Health.

[30]  Robert S. Wyer,et al.  Effects of Country-of-Origin and Product-Attribute Information on Product Evaluation: An Information Processing Perspective , 1989 .

[31]  S. Dopson,et al.  Inter-epistemic Power and Transforming Knowledge Objects in a Biomedical Network , 2010 .

[32]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[33]  E. Ferlie,et al.  The Nonspread of Innovations: the Mediating Role of Professionals , 2005 .

[34]  Andy Haines,et al.  Learning from the Brazilian Community Health Worker Model in North Wales , 2013, Globalization and Health.

[35]  N. Hoffart Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory , 2000 .

[36]  Brett A. S. Martin,et al.  Countering negative country of origin effects using imagery processing , 2011 .

[37]  H. Swartz,et al.  Managing the global burden of depression: lessons from the developing world. , 2003, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[38]  D. Baldwin,et al.  The future of antidepressant pharmacotherapy. , 2003, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.

[39]  N. Dogra,et al.  Learning from low income countries: what are the lessons?: Partnerships in mental health are possible without multidisciplinary teams , 2004, British medical journal.

[40]  N. Dogra,et al.  Partnerships in mental health are possible without multidisciplinary teams [letter] , 2004 .

[41]  Robert E Black,et al.  Effect of community-based behaviour change management on neonatal mortality in Shivgarh, Uttar Pradesh, India: a cluster-randomised controlled trial , 2008, The Lancet.

[42]  E. Susser,et al.  Topics for our times: can we learn from the care of persons with mental illness in developing countries? , 1996, American journal of public health.

[43]  S. Huttly,et al.  Effect of community-based peer counsellors on exclusive breastfeeding practices in Dhaka, Bangladesh: a randomised controlled trial , 2000, The Lancet.