Assessment of predation risk through referential communication in incubating birds

Parents of many bird species produce alarm calls when they approach and deter a nest predator in order to defend their offspring. Alarm calls have been shown to warn nestlings about predatory threats, but parents also face a similar risk of predation when incubating eggs in their nests. Here, I show that incubating female Japanese great tits, Parus minor, assess predation risk by conspecific alarm calls given outside the nest cavity. Tits produce acoustically discrete alarm calls for different nest predators: “jar” calls for snakes and “chicka” calls for other predators such as crows and martens. Playback experiments revealed that incubating females responded to “jar” calls by leaving their nest, whereas they responded to “chicka” calls by looking out of the nest entrance. Since snakes invade the nest cavity, escaping from the nest helps females avoid snake predation. In contrast, “chicka” calls are used for a variety of predator types, and therefore, looking out of the nest entrance helps females gather information about the type and location of approaching predators. These results show that incubating females derive information about predator type from different types of alarm calls, providing a novel example of functionally referential communication.

[1]  Burt P. Kotler Antipredator Defenses in Birds and Mammals, Tim Caro. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (2005), pp. xv+591. Price £27.00 paperback , 2006 .

[2]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. , 2005, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[3]  Toshitaka N. Suzuki,et al.  Mobbing Calls of Japanese Tits Signal Predator Type: Field Observations of Natural Predator Encounters , 2013 .

[4]  D. Holdstock Past, present--and future? , 2005, Medicine, conflict, and survival.

[5]  S. Gill,et al.  Nest Defence By Yellow Warblers: Recognition of a Brood Parasite and an Avian Nest Predator , 1996 .

[6]  Michael Griesser,et al.  Referential Calls Signal Predator Behavior in a Group-Living Bird Species , 2008, Current Biology.

[7]  Alarm calls of the Cyprus Wheatear Oenanthe cypriaca—one for nest defence, one for parent–offspring communication? , 2013, acta ethologica.

[8]  M. Manser,et al.  Banded mongoose recruitment calls convey information about risk and not stimulus type , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[9]  M. Manser,et al.  The acoustic structure of suricates' alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[10]  R. D. Magrath,et al.  Parental alarm calls suppress nestling vocalization , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[11]  M. Griesser,et al.  Incubating females use dynamic risk assessment to evaluate the risk posed by different predators , 2013 .

[12]  A. Meillère,et al.  Impact of chronic noise exposure on antipredator behavior: an experiment in breeding house sparrows , 2015 .

[13]  Erick Greene,et al.  Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.

[14]  T. E. Martin,et al.  Nest predation increases with parental activity: separating nest site and parental activity effects , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  Robert D. Magrath,et al.  Calling in the Face of Danger: Predation Risk and Acoustic Communication by Parent Birds and Their Offspring , 2010 .

[16]  T. E. Martin,et al.  Avian Life History Evolution in Relation to Nest Sites, Nest Predation, and Food , 1995 .

[17]  Donald E. Kroodsma,et al.  Pseudoreplication in playback experiments, revisited a decade later , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[18]  G. J. Fernández,et al.  Alarm calls of the Southern House Wren Troglodytes musculus: variation with nesting stage and predator model , 2009, Journal of Ornithology.

[19]  R. D. Magrath,et al.  Calling at a cost: elevated nestling calling attracts predators to active nests , 2011, Biology Letters.

[20]  G. J. Fernández,et al.  Reduction of nestlings’ vocalizations in response to parental alarm calls in the Southern House Wren, Troglodytes musculus , 2011, Journal of Ornithology.

[21]  M. Manser,et al.  The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[22]  R. Ricklefs An analysis of nesting mortality in birds , 1969 .

[23]  J. Masero,et al.  Predation risk on incubating adults constrains the choice of thermally favourable nest sites in a plover , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  Toshitaka N. Suzuki,et al.  Observations of Predation Attempts on Avian Nest Boxes by Japanese Martens (Martes Melampus) , 2013 .

[25]  Toshitaka N. Suzuki Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats , 2011, Current Biology.

[26]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[27]  Toshitaka N. Suzuki Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[28]  Andrea M.-K. Bierema,et al.  On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling , 2013 .

[29]  T. Albrecht,et al.  Nest Crypsis, Reproductive Value of a Clutch and Escape Decisions in Incubating Female Mallards Anas platyrhynchos , 2004 .

[30]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[31]  Simon W. Townsend,et al.  Functionally Referential Communication in Mammals: The Past, Present and the Future , 2013 .

[32]  Dirk Platzen,et al.  Adaptive differences in response to two types of parental alarm call in altricial nestlings , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Toshitaka N. Suzuki Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[34]  S. Gill,et al.  Functional reference in an alarm signal given during nest defence: seet calls of yellow warblers denote brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbirds , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[35]  Junko Kondo,et al.  From nestling calls to fledgling silence: adaptive timing of change in response to aerial alarm calls , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.