Long-Term Memory for Haptically Explored Objects: Fidelity, Durability, Incidental Encoding, and Cross-Modal Transfer

The question of how many of our perceptual experiences are stored in long-term memory has received considerable attention. The present study examined long-term memory for haptic experiences. Blindfolded participants haptically explored 168 everyday objects (e.g., a pen) for 10 s each. In a blindfolded memory test, they indicated which of two objects from the same basic-level category (e.g., two different pens) had been touched before. As shown in Experiment 1 (N = 26), memory was nearly perfect when tested immediately after exploration (94%) and still high when tested after 1 week (85%). As shown in Experiment 2 (N = 43), when participants explored the objects without the intention to memorize them, memory in a 1-week delayed surprise test was still high (79%), even when assessed with a cross-modal visual memory test (73%). These results indicate that detailed, durable, long-term memory representations are stored as a natural product of haptic perception.

[1]  R L Klatzky,et al.  Identifying objects by touch: An “expert system” , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  Nathan S. Rose,et al.  Recognition without awareness: Encoding and retrieval factors. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[3]  Jeffrey S. Bowers,et al.  Detailed and gist-like visual memories are forgotten at similar rates over the course of a week , 2015, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[4]  L. Standing Learning 10,000 pictures. , 1973, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  Emily W. Bushnell,et al.  Children's haptic and cross-modal recognition with familiar and unfamiliar objects. , 1999 .

[6]  S. Lacey,et al.  Visuo-haptic multisensory object recognition, categorization, and representation , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[7]  Susan Haag,et al.  Delayed memory for visual—haptic exploration of familiar objects , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Marcia K. Johnson A multiple-entry, modular memory system , 1983 .

[9]  Carrick C. Williams,et al.  Incidental visual memory for targets and distractors in visual search , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Haptic Shape Processing in Visual Cortex , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Joel L. Voss,et al.  Accurate forced-choice recognition without awareness of memory retrieval. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[12]  Philipp Spachtholz,et al.  High-Fidelity Visual Long-Term Memory within an Unattended Blink of an Eye , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[13]  M. J. O. Andrade,et al.  Haptic memory and forgetting: a systematic review , 2013 .

[14]  Aude Oliva,et al.  Visual long-term memory has a massive storage capacity for object details , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[15]  J. Henderson,et al.  Incidental visual memory for objects in scenes , 2005 .

[16]  E. Bushnell,et al.  Children's haptic and cross-modal recognition with familiar and unfamiliar objects. , 1999, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.

[17]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Some thoughts on the interaction between perception and reflection , 2012 .

[18]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  Scene Memory Is More Detailed Than You Think : The Role of Categories in Visual Long-Term Memory , 2010 .

[19]  H. Egeth,et al.  Massive memory revisited: Limitations on storage capacity for object details in visual long-term memory , 2015, Learning & memory.

[20]  S. Vogt,et al.  Long-term memory for 400 pictures on a common theme. , 2007, Experimental psychology.

[21]  Scott A. Guerin,et al.  Retrieval Failure Contributes to Gist-Based False Recognition. , 2012, Journal of memory and language.