Learning pop-out detection: building representations for conflicting target-distractor relationships

Studies of perceptual learning consistently found that improvement is stimulus specific. These findings were interpreted as indicating an early cortical learning site. In line with this interpretation, we consider two alternative hypotheses: the 'earliest modification' and the 'output-level modification' assumptions, which respectively assume that learning occurs within the earliest representation which is selective for the trained stimuli, or at cortical levels receiving its output. We studied performance in a pop-out task using light bar distractor elements of one orientation, and a target element rotated by 30 degrees (or 90 degrees). We tested the alternative hypotheses by examining pop-out learning through an initial training phase, a subsequent learning stage with swapped target and distracted orientations, and a final re-test with the originally trained stimuli. We found learning does not transfer across orientation swapping. However, following training with swapped orientations, a similar performance level is reached as with original orientations. That is, learning neither facilitates nor interferes to a substantial degree with subsequent performance with altered stimuli. Furthermore, this re-training does not hamper performance with the original trained stimuli. If training changed the earliest orientation selective representation (specializing it for performance of the particular performed task) it would necessarily affect performance with swapped orientations, as well. The co-existence of similar asymptotes for apparently conflicting stimulus sets refutes the 'earliest modification' hypothesis, supporting the alternative 'output level modification' hypothesis. We conclude that secondary cortical processing levels use outputs from the earliest orientation representation to compute higher order structures, promoting and improving successful task performance.

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  H. Nothdurft Sensitivity for structure gradient in texture discrimination tasks , 1985, Vision Research.

[3]  J. Maunsell,et al.  Visual effects of lesions of cortical area V2 in macaques , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Some results on translation invariance in the human visual system. , 1990, Spatial vision.

[5]  Spatial anisotropy in feature search performance and learning , 1994 .

[6]  B. Julesz,et al.  Short-range limitation on detection of feature differences. , 1987, Spatial vision.

[7]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[8]  Learning is both stimulus and task specific , 1991 .

[9]  Quick Rf A vector-magnitude model of contrast detection. , 1974 .

[10]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Direction-specific improvement in motion discrimination , 1987, Vision Research.

[11]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  A. Treisman,et al.  Automaticity and preattentive processing. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[13]  D. Scott Perceptual learning. , 1974, Queen's nursing journal.

[14]  G. Orban,et al.  Human perceptual learning in identifying the oblique orientation: retinotopy, orientation specificity and monocularity. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  R. Frostig,et al.  Cortical point-spread function and long-range lateral interactions revealed by real-time optical imaging of macaque monkey primary visual cortex , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  How early is early vision? Evidence from perceptual learning , 1993 .

[17]  M. Ahissar,et al.  Plasticity in auditory cortical circuitry , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[19]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Learning Pop-out Detection: Specificities to Stimulus Characteristics , 1996, Vision Research.

[20]  Ruxandra Sireteanu,et al.  Perceptual learning in visual search: Fast, enduring, but non-specific , 1995, Vision Research.

[21]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning , 1997, Nature.

[22]  Abrupt learning in illusory contour perception , 1996 .

[23]  H. Pashler,et al.  Improvement in line orientation discrimination is retinally local but dependent on cognitive set , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  H Sompolinsky,et al.  Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Handbook of Neuropsychology , 1991 .

[26]  D. Levi,et al.  Perceptual learning in parafoveal vision , 1995, Vision Research.

[27]  R. Sekuler,et al.  A specific and enduring improvement in visual motion discrimination. , 1982, Science.

[28]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[29]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Abrupt learning and retinal size specificity in illusory-contour perception , 1997, Current Biology.

[30]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex , 1959, The Journal of physiology.

[31]  A. Treisman Perceiving and re-perceiving objects. , 1992, The American psychologist.

[32]  A J O'Toole,et al.  Learning to See Random-Dot Stereograms , 1992, Perception.

[33]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[34]  B. Kröse,et al.  The control and speed of shifts of attention , 1989, Vision Research.

[35]  T. Poggio,et al.  Fast perceptual learning in hyperacuity , 1995, Vision Research.

[36]  D Sagi,et al.  Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  M Ahissar,et al.  The role of attention in early perceptual learning , 1993 .

[38]  Shimon Edelman,et al.  Models of Perceptual Learning in Vernier Hyperacuity , 1993, Neural Computation.

[39]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[40]  M. Brill Perceptual Constancy: Why Things Look As They Do , 1999 .

[41]  O Braddick,et al.  Orientation-Specific Learning in Stereopsis , 1973, Perception.

[42]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[43]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Perceptual learning transfer over space and orientation , 1996 .

[44]  B. Julesz Textons, the elements of texture perception, and their interactions , 1981, Nature.

[45]  A. Fiorentini,et al.  Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency , 1980, Nature.

[46]  J. Wolfe,et al.  The role of categorization in visual search for orientation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  T Poggio,et al.  Fast perceptual learning in visual hyperacuity. , 1991, Science.

[48]  T. Papathomas Early vision and beyond , 1995 .

[49]  U Polat,et al.  Spatial interactions in human vision: from near to far via experience-dependent cascades of connections. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[50]  Tomaso A. Poggio,et al.  Learning of visual modules from examples: A framework for understanding adaptive visual performance , 1992, CVGIP Image Underst..

[51]  E. Gibson Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development , 1969 .

[52]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Perceptual Constancy: why things look as they do , 1998 .