Smoking and human information processing

There is much evidence which indicates that smoking improves various aspects of human information processing (Wesnes 1987). The aim of the present study was to elucidate the stages of human information processing which are improved after cigarette smoking. Twelve regular smokers were tested on three cognitive tasks using a repeated measures design. Tasks used were: rapid visual information processing (RVIP), digit symbol substitution (DSST), and inspection time (IT). Performance parameters derived from these were intended to index different stages of the information processing sequence. Only those measures which involved a motor component were improved after smoking: response time on the RVIP task (P<0.025) and DSST performance (P<0.1). These findings suggest that central cholinergic pathways are involved in the late, response-related stages of the processing sequence.

[1]  D. Warburton,et al.  Neurochemistry of behaviour. , 1981, British medical bulletin.

[2]  Gunn Johansson,et al.  Changes in behavioral and physiological activation induced by cigarette smoking in habitual smokers , 2004, Psychopharmacologia.

[3]  T. Nettelbeck Inspection time and intelligence. , 1987 .

[4]  T Nettelbeck,et al.  Perceptual Indices of Performance: The Measurement of ‘Inspection Time’ and ‘Noise’ in the Visual System , 1972, Perception.

[5]  I. Hindmarch,et al.  Human psychopharmacology : measures and methods , 1990 .

[6]  M. M. Taylor,et al.  PEST: Efficient Estimates on Probability Functions , 1967 .

[7]  Ian J. Deary,et al.  Evoked potential correlates of inspection time. , 1989 .

[8]  Douglas Vickers,et al.  The rationale for the inspection time index. , 1986 .

[9]  Gunn Johansson,et al.  Behavioural and physiological effects of cigarette smoking in a monotonous situation , 2004, Psychopharmacologia.

[10]  A. Sanders Towards a model of stress and human performance. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[11]  R C Atkinson,et al.  Individual differences and interrelationships among a select set of cognitive skills , 1976, Memory & cognition.

[12]  Keith Wesnes,et al.  Effects of scopolamine and nicotine on human rapid information processing performance , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[13]  Raymond E. Thornton,et al.  Smoking behaviour: Physiological and psychological influences , 1978 .

[14]  Norman W. Heimstra,et al.  EFFECTS OF SMOKING UPON SUSTAINED PERFORMANCE IN A SIMULATED DRIVING TASK * , 1967 .

[15]  Hans J. Eysenck,et al.  Intelligence, reaction time and the effects of smoking , 1988 .

[16]  K. Wesnes,et al.  Effects of scopolamine on stimulus sensitivity and response bias in a visual vigilance task. , 1983, Neuropsychobiology.

[17]  I. Deary,et al.  Inspection time and intelligence , 2001 .

[18]  K. Wesnes,et al.  Effects of nicotine on stimulus sensitivity and response bias in a visual vigilance task. , 1983, Neuropsychobiology.

[19]  D. G. Williams,et al.  Effects of cigarette smoking on immediate memory and performance in different kinds of smoker. , 1980, British journal of psychology.

[20]  A. Jensen,et al.  Process differences and individual differences in some cognitive tasks , 1987 .

[21]  D E Broadbent,et al.  Performance and its measurement. , 1984, British journal of clinical pharmacology.

[22]  K. Wesnes,et al.  Effects of smoking on rapid information processing performance. , 1983, Neuropsychobiology.

[23]  K. Wesnes,et al.  The effects of cigarettes of varying yield on rapid information processing performance , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[24]  D. Wechsler Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. , 1955 .

[25]  G. Preston,et al.  Modelling dementia: Effects of scopolamine on memory and attention , 1988, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  Keith Wesnes,et al.  Nicotine Increases Mental Efficiency: But How? , 1987 .