Oculomotor freezing reflects tactile temporal expectation and aids tactile perception

The oculomotor system keeps the eyes steady in expectation of visual events. Here, recording microsaccades while people performed a tactile, frequency discrimination task enabled us to test whether the oculomotor system shows an analogous preparatory response for unrelated tactile events. We manipulated the temporal predictability of tactile targets using tactile cues, which preceded the target by either constant (high predictability) or variable (low predictability) time intervals. We find that microsaccades are inhibited prior to tactile targets and more so for constant than variable intervals, revealing a tight crossmodal link between tactile temporal expectation and oculomotor action. These findings portray oculomotor freezing as a marker of crossmodal temporal expectation. Moreover, microsaccades occurring around the tactile target presentation are associated with reduced task performance, suggesting that oculomotor freezing mitigates potential detrimental, concomitant effects of microsaccades and revealing a crossmodal coupling between tactile perception and oculomotor action. The oculomotor system keeps the eyes steady in expectation of visual events, inhibiting small fixational eye movements. Here, the authors reveal that this oculomotor freezing reflects tactile temporal expectations and aids tactile perception.

[1]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Oculomotor inhibition reflects temporal expectations , 2019, NeuroImage.

[2]  H Burton,et al.  Attending to and Remembering Tactile Stimuli: A Review of Brain Imaging Data and Single-Neuron Responses , 2000, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[3]  S. Martinez-Conde,et al.  The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[4]  Salvador Soto-Faraco,et al.  Cross-modal decoupling in temporal attention between audition and touch , 2019, Psychological research.

[5]  Francisco M. Costela,et al.  Task difficulty in mental arithmetic affects microsaccadic rates and magnitudes , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  Luigi Tamè,et al.  Microsaccades distinguish between global and local visual processing , 2007, Neuroreport.

[7]  Daniel P. Newman,et al.  Antagonistic Interactions Between Microsaccades and Evidence Accumulation Processes During Decision Formation , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Emotional faces guide the eyes in the absence of awareness , 2019, eLife.

[9]  Steeve Zozor,et al.  Microsaccades are modulated by both attentional demands of a visual discrimination task and background noise. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[10]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[11]  Matteo Valsecchi,et al.  Microsaccadic responses in a bimodal oddball task , 2009, Psychological research.

[12]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Orienting Attention to Points in Time Improves Stimulus Processing Both within and across Modalities , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Christian N L Olivers,et al.  Oculomotor measures reveal the temporal dynamics of preparing for search. , 2017, Progress in brain research.

[14]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Attention Reverses the Effect of Prediction in Silencing Sensory Signals , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  A Neural Mechanism for Microsaccade Generation in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2009, Science.

[17]  M Concetta Morrone,et al.  Saccadic eye movements cause compression of time as well as space , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  Nuttida Rungratsameetaweemana,et al.  Dissociating the impact of attention and expectation on early sensory processing. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[19]  Erich Schröger,et al.  Microsaccadic Responses Indicate Fast Categorization of Sounds: A Novel Approach to Study Auditory Cognition , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Directing Voluntary Temporal Attention Increases Fixational Stability , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Marcus Nyström,et al.  Fixational eye movements predict visual sensitivity , 2015, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Dekel Abeles,et al.  Temporal dynamics of saccades explained by a self-paced process , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[24]  John E. Schlerf,et al.  Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  B. L. Zuber,et al.  Saccadic suppression: elevation of visual threshold associated with saccadic eye movements. , 1966, Experimental neurology.

[26]  A. Nobre,et al.  Time in Cortical Circuits , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Crossmodal coupling of oculomotor control and spatial attention in vision and audition , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Hong-Jin Sun,et al.  Modulation of microsaccade rate by task difficulty revealed through between- and within-trial comparisons. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[29]  Rachel N. Denison,et al.  Temporal attention improves perception similarly at foveal and parafoveal locations , 2019, Journal of vision.

[30]  S. Holm A Simple Sequentially Rejective Multiple Test Procedure , 1979 .

[31]  M. Morrone,et al.  Vision During Saccadic Eye Movements. , 2018, Annual review of vision science.

[32]  Karen T. Navarro,et al.  Modality-specific attention attenuates visual-tactile integration and recalibration effects by reducing prior expectations of a common source for vision and touch , 2020, Cognition.

[33]  Gregory C. DeAngelis,et al.  Bridging the gap between theories of sensory cue integration and the physiology of multisensory neurons , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Computational Modeling of Collicular Integration of Perceptual Responses and Attention in Microsaccades , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Tracking Without Perceiving , 2011, Psychology Science.

[36]  M. Turatto,et al.  Visual oddballs induce prolonged microsaccadic inhibition , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  C. Summerfield,et al.  Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  Martina Poletti,et al.  Control and Functions of Fixational Eye Movements. , 2015, Annual review of vision science.

[39]  Michael B. McCamy,et al.  Microsaccade and drift dynamics reflect mental fatigue , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[40]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Prestimulus Inhibition of Saccades in Adults With and Without Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder as an Index of Temporal Expectations , 2017, Psychological science.

[41]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  More is not always better: adaptive gain control explains dissociation between perception and action , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[42]  B. Röder,et al.  Feeling a Touch to the Hand on the Foot , 2019, Current Biology.

[43]  Todd M. Herrington,et al.  The Effect of Microsaccades on the Correlation between Neural Activity and Behavior in Middle Temporal, Ventral Intraparietal, and Lateral Intraparietal Areas , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  T. Stanford,et al.  Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  Ziad M Hafed,et al.  Superior colliculus inactivation alters the relationship between covert visual attention and microsaccades , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[46]  Anna C. Nobre,et al.  Anticipated moments: temporal structure in attention , 2017, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  Y. Bonneh,et al.  Familiarity revealed by involuntary eye movements on the fringe of awareness , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[48]  R. Steinman,et al.  Voluntary Control of Microsaccades during Maintained Monocular Fixation , 1967, Science.

[49]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  An integrated model of fixational eye movements and microsaccades , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[50]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Similar Effects of Feature-Based Attention on Motion Perception and Pursuit Eye Movements at Different Levels of Awareness , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  M. Rolfs Microsaccades: Small steps on a long way , 2009, Vision Research.

[52]  C. Cierpka,et al.  Particle imaging techniques for volumetric three-component (3D3C) velocity measurements in microfluidics , 2011, Journal of Visualization.

[53]  Lance M. Optican,et al.  Modeling the Triggering of Saccades, Microsaccades, and Saccadic Intrusions , 2018, Front. Neurol..

[54]  Giovanni Galfano,et al.  Working memory load modulates microsaccadic rate. , 2017, Journal of vision.

[55]  M. Turatto,et al.  Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades , 2006, Neuroreport.

[56]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[57]  Xoana G. Troncoso,et al.  Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[58]  J. Braun,et al.  Rare but precious: Microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation , 2010, Vision Research.

[59]  B. Stein,et al.  Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  Martin Rolfs,et al.  Oculomotor inhibition covaries with conscious detection. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[61]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[62]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Selective attention within the foveola , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[63]  R. Veit,et al.  BOLD adaptation in vibrotactile stimulation: neuronal networks involved in frequency discrimination. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  Julian Keil,et al.  Temporal orienting precedes intersensory attention and has opposing effects on early evoked brain activity , 2017, NeuroImage.

[65]  Reinhold Kliegl,et al.  Microsaccadic modulation of response times in spatial attention tasks , 2009, Psychological research.

[66]  Ralf Engbert Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[67]  Marisa Carrasco,et al.  Attention flexibly trades off across points in time , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[68]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Oculomotor inhibition precedes temporally expected auditory targets , 2020, Nature Communications.

[69]  D. Heeger,et al.  Spontaneous Microsaccades Reflect Shifts in Covert Attention , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[70]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Toward a model of microsaccade generation: the case of microsaccadic inhibition. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[71]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Acting without seeing: eye movements reveal visual processing without awareness , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[72]  R. Leigh,et al.  Triggering mechanisms in microsaccade and saccade generation: a novel proposal , 2011, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[73]  YU Peng,et al.  Time compression of visual perception around microsaccades. , 2017, Journal of neurophysiology.

[74]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Neuronal Activity in the Lateral Intraparietal Area and Spatial Attention , 2003, Science.

[75]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Intersensory selective attention and temporal orienting operate in parallel and are instantiated in spatially distinct sensory and motor cortices , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[76]  K. Fujii,et al.  Visualization for the analysis of fluid motion , 2005, J. Vis..

[77]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Dissociable prior influences of signal probability and relevance on visual contrast sensitivity , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[78]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  Is neocortex essentially multisensory? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[79]  Dekel Abeles,et al.  Transient and sustained effects of stimulus properties on the generation of microsaccades. , 2019, Journal of vision.

[80]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[81]  Ziad M. Hafed,et al.  Microsaccadic Suppression of Visual Bursts in the Primate Superior Colliculus , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience.