The evaluative role of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex in rule-based category learning

&NA; Category learning is a critical neurobiological function that allows organisms to simplify a complex world. Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex (rlPFC) is often active in neurobiological studies of category learning; however, the specific role this region serves in category learning remains uncertain. Previous category learning studies have hypothesized that the rlPFC is involved in switching between rules, whereas others have emphasized rule abstraction and evaluation. We aimed to clarify the role of rlPFC in category learning and dissociate switching and evaluation accounts using two common types of category learning tasks: matching and classification. The matching task involved matching a reference stimulus to one of four target stimuli. In the classification task, participants were shown a single stimulus and learned to classify it into one of two categories. Matching and classification are similar but place different demands on switching and evaluation. In matching, a rule can be known with certainty after a single correct answer. In classification, participants may need to evaluate evidence for a rule even after an initial correct response. This critical difference allows isolation of evaluative functions from switching functions. If the rlPFC is primarily involved in switching between representations, it should cease to be active once participants settle on a given rule in both tasks. If the rlPFC is involved in rule evaluation, its activation should persist in the classification task, but not matching. The results revealed that rlPFC activation persisted into correct trials in classification, but not matching, suggesting that it continues to be involved in the evaluations of evidence for a rule even after participants have arrived at the correct rule. HighlightsDifferences between rule‐based matching and classification tasks were highlighted.Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex remains engaged past when participants actively switch between rules.Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex is involved in evaluation of evidence for a rule in rule‐based category learning tasks.

[1]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Left, but not right, rostrolateral prefrontal cortex meets a stringent test of the relational integration hypothesis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[2]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Cortical and subcortical brain regions involved in rule-based category learning , 2005, Neuroreport.

[3]  Shawn W. Ell,et al.  The neurobiology of human category learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Neural networks supporting switching, hypothesis testing, and rule application , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Daniel Elmore,et al.  How good is it , 1998 .

[6]  Shawn W. Ell,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Rule-based and Information-integration Category Learning , 2022 .

[7]  M. Gluck,et al.  Interactive memory systems in the human brain , 2001, Nature.

[8]  Iroise Dumontheil,et al.  The gateway hypothesis of rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Thomas F. Nugent,et al.  Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  Ninon Burgos,et al.  New advances in the Clinica software platform for clinical neuroimaging studies , 2019 .

[11]  M. Buckley,et al.  Essential functions of primate frontopolar cortex in cognition , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[12]  C. Chabris,et al.  Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Turner,et al.  Deficient approaches to human neuroimaging , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Tyler Davis,et al.  From Concrete Examples to Abstract Relations: The Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Integrates Novel Examples into Relational Categories , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Confounds in multivariate pattern analysis: Theory and rule representation case study , 2013, NeuroImage.

[16]  W. K. Simmons,et al.  Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Shawn W. Ell,et al.  Focal putamen lesions impair learning in rule-based, but not information-integration categorization tasks , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  David Badre,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence for a Hierarchical Organization of the Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction , 2007, Magnetic Resonance Materials in Physics, Biology and Medicine.

[20]  Hans Knutsson,et al.  Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[22]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Reinforcement Learning in Multidimensional Environments Relies on Attention Mechanisms , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Inferring Relevance in a Changing World , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[24]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Dynamics of frontal, striatal, and hippocampal systems during rule learning. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  T. Braver,et al.  BOLD Correlates of Trial-by-Trial Reaction Time Variability in Gray and White Matter: A Multi-Study fMRI Analysis , 2009, PloS one.

[26]  S. Fleming,et al.  Domain-specific impairment in metacognitive accuracy following anterior prefrontal lesions , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Dissociable Prototype Learning Systems: Evidence from Brain Imaging and Behavior , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Permutation inference for the general linear model , 2014, NeuroImage.

[29]  Stephen Lawrie,et al.  Functional Specialization within Rostral Prefrontal Cortex (Area 10): A Meta-analysis , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[30]  R. Heaton,et al.  The utility of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in detecting and localizing frontal lobe lesions. , 1980, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[31]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest , 2006, NeuroImage.

[32]  R. Dolan,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Metacognition in Perceptual Decision Making , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Altered modular organization of intrinsic brain functional networks in patients with Parkinson’s disease , 2017, Brain Imaging and Behavior.

[34]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Category learning in the brain. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[35]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Neural correlates of rule-based and information-integration visual category learning. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  How do the basal ganglia contribute to categorization? Their roles in generalization, response selection, and learning via feedback , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  A Computational Model of Event Segmentation From Perceptual Prediction , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[38]  B. Love,et al.  Learning the exception to the rule: model-based FMRI reveals specialized representations for surprising category members. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Robust group analysis using outlier inference , 2008, NeuroImage.

[40]  Carter Wendelken,et al.  Transitive Inference: Distinct Contributions of Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex and the Hippocampus , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  B. Forstmann,et al.  Brain networks of perceptual decision-making: an fMRI ALE meta-analysis , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[42]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[43]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Recruitment of anterior dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human reasoning: a parametric study of relational complexity. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  David L. Faigman,et al.  Human category learning. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[45]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[46]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Multiple systems of category learning , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[47]  A. Owen,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  小羽 俊士,et al.  Wisconsin card sorting test , 2013 .

[49]  Anders M. Dale,et al.  Cortical Surface-Based Analysis I. Segmentation and Surface Reconstruction , 1999, NeuroImage.

[50]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data , 2001, NeuroImage.

[51]  J. Hirsch,et al.  A Neural Representation of Categorization Uncertainty in the Human Brain , 2006, Neuron.

[52]  Jonathan W. Peirce,et al.  PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[53]  Gui Xue,et al.  Global Neural Pattern Similarity as a Common Basis for Categorization and Recognition Memory , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Multilevel linear modelling for FMRI group analysis using Bayesian inference , 2004, NeuroImage.

[55]  James K. Kroger,et al.  Rostrolateral Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Relational Integration during Reasoning , 2001, NeuroImage.

[56]  P. Dayan,et al.  Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in humans , 2006, Nature.

[57]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  B. Love,et al.  Striatal and hippocampal entropy and recognition signals in category learning: simultaneous processes revealed by model-based fMRI. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[59]  R. Shepard,et al.  Learning and memorization of classifications. , 1961 .

[60]  R. Dolan,et al.  Effects of loss aversion on post-decision wagering: Implications for measures of awareness , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[61]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Hemispheric asymmetry in human lateral prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[62]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[63]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[64]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL , 2009, NeuroImage.

[65]  C. White,et al.  Perceptual Criteria in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[66]  Gereon R Fink,et al.  Disentangling the prefrontal network for rule selection by means of a non‐verbal variant of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[67]  Catherine Jones Wisconsin card sorting test , 2013 .

[68]  A. Graybiel Habits, rituals, and the evaluative brain. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[69]  Silvia A. Bunge,et al.  Evolutionary and Developmental Changes in the Lateral Frontoparietal Network: A Little Goes a Long Way for Higher-Level Cognition , 2014, Neuron.

[70]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Category learning and the memory systems debate , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[71]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[72]  Noah A. Shamosh,et al.  Frontopolar cortex mediates abstract integration in analogy , 2006, Brain Research.

[73]  K. Amunts,et al.  Spatial organization of neurons in the frontal pole sets humans apart from great apes. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[74]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Prefrontal cortex organization: dissociating effects of temporal abstraction, relational abstraction, and integration with FMRI. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[75]  J. Cerella,et al.  Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[76]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Distinct mechanisms in visual category learning , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[77]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[78]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[79]  M. Petrides,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting Revisited: Distinct Neural Circuits Participating in Different Stages of the Task Identified by Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[80]  Bradley C. Love,et al.  Two pathways to stimulus encoding in category learning? , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[81]  E. Bullmore,et al.  How Good Is Good Enough in Path Analysis of fMRI Data? , 2000, NeuroImage.

[82]  J. Kruschke,et al.  ALCOVE: an exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. , 1992, Psychological review.

[83]  F Gregory Ashby,et al.  A role for the perceptual representation memory system in category learning , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[84]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Rule-plus-exception model of classification learning. , 1994, Psychological review.

[85]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. , 1998, Psychological review.

[86]  Bruce Fischl,et al.  Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration , 2009, NeuroImage.

[87]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences Human Category Learning 2.0 Brief Review of First-generation Research , 2022 .

[88]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference , 2009, NeuroImage.

[89]  Francisco Barceló,et al.  Task Switching and Novelty Processing Activate a Common Neural Network for Cognitive Control , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[90]  Jonathan Westley Peirce,et al.  Neuroinformatics Original Research Article Generating Stimuli for Neuroscience Using Psychopy , 2022 .

[91]  C. Wendelken,et al.  Rostrolateral prefrontal cortex: Domain‐general or domain‐sensitive? , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[92]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Anterior prefrontal cortex mediates rule learning in humans. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[93]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  General multilevel linear modeling for group analysis in FMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[94]  Satrajit S. Ghosh,et al.  Nipype: A Flexible, Lightweight and Extensible Neuroimaging Data Processing Framework in Python , 2011, Front. Neuroinform..

[95]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[96]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  Categorical evidence, confidence, and urgency during probabilistic categorization , 2016, NeuroImage.

[97]  R Turner,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[98]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[99]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Quantifying the internal structure of categories using a neural typicality measure. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[100]  A. Brian,et al.  ANTs/ANTsR Brain Templates , 2014 .

[101]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Learned Predictions of Error Likelihood in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2005, Science.

[102]  Tomiki Sumiyoshi,et al.  Role of , 2017 .

[103]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Statistical improvements in functional magnetic resonance imaging analyses produced by censoring high‐motion data points , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[104]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Handbook of Functional MRI Data Analysis: Visualizing, localizing, and reporting fMRI data , 2011 .

[105]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Frontal Cortex and the Discovery of Abstract Action Rules , 2010, Neuron.