Hierarchical decision processes that operate over distinct timescales underlie choice and changes in strategy

Significance Decisions are guided by available information and strategies that link information to action. Following a bad outcome, two potential sources of error—flawed strategy and poor information—must be distinguished to improve future performance. In a direction discrimination task where subjects decide by accumulating sensory evidence to a bound, we show that humans disambiguate sources of error by integrating expected accuracy and outcome over multiple choices. The strategy switches when the integral reaches a threshold. A hierarchy of decision processes in which lower levels integrate sensory evidence over short timescales, and higher levels interact with lower levels over longer timescales, quantitatively explains the behavior. Expected accuracy links these two levels and enables adaptive changes of decision strategy. Decision-making in a natural environment depends on a hierarchy of interacting decision processes. A high-level strategy guides ongoing choices, and the outcomes of those choices determine whether or not the strategy should change. When the right decision strategy is uncertain, as in most natural settings, feedback becomes ambiguous because negative outcomes may be due to limited information or bad strategy. Disambiguating the cause of feedback requires active inference and is key to updating the strategy. We hypothesize that the expected accuracy of a choice plays a crucial rule in this inference, and setting the strategy depends on integration of outcome and expectations across choices. We test this hypothesis with a task in which subjects report the net direction of random dot kinematograms with varying difficulty while the correct stimulus−response association undergoes invisible and unpredictable switches every few trials. We show that subjects treat negative feedback as evidence for a switch but weigh it with their expected accuracy. Subjects accumulate switch evidence (in units of log-likelihood ratio) across trials and update their response strategy when accumulated evidence reaches a bound. A computational framework based on these principles quantitatively explains all aspects of the behavior, providing a plausible neural mechanism for the implementation of hierarchical multiscale decision processes. We suggest that a similar neural computation—bounded accumulation of evidence—underlies both the choice and switches in the strategy that govern the choice, and that expected accuracy of a choice represents a key link between the levels of the decision-making hierarchy.

[1]  J. Jastrow,et al.  On small differences in sensation , 1884 .

[2]  V. Henmon,et al.  The relation of the time of a judgment to its accuracy. , 1911 .

[3]  J. Wolfowitz,et al.  Optimum Character of the Sequential Probability Ratio Test , 1948 .

[4]  K. Lashley The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .

[5]  J. Fleiss,et al.  Effect of stimulus uncertainty on the pupillary dilation response and the vertex evoked potential. , 1973, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  D. Vickers Decision processes in visual perception , 1979 .

[7]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: analysis of decision under risk , 1979 .

[8]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[9]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[10]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  E. Crone,et al.  Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecendents and consequences of decision making. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Angela J. Yu,et al.  Uncertainty, Neuromodulation, and Attention , 2005, Neuron.

[15]  P. Dayan,et al.  Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  H. Seung,et al.  JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2005, 84, 581–617 NUMBER 3(NOVEMBER) LINEAR-NONLINEAR-POISSON MODELS OF PRIMATE CHOICE DYNAMICS , 2022 .

[17]  J. Ditterich Evidence for time‐variant decision making , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[19]  A. Tversky,et al.  Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk — Source link , 2007 .

[20]  Aaron C. Courville,et al.  The rat as particle filter , 2007, NIPS.

[21]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Learning the value of information in an uncertain world , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  Karl J. Friston Hierarchical Models in the Brain , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[24]  Hatim A. Zariwala,et al.  Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence , 2008, Nature.

[25]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision-making with multiple alternatives , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Hierarchically organized behavior and its neural foundations: A reinforcement learning perspective , 2009, Cognition.

[27]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[28]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  Detecting and predicting changes , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.

[29]  C. Law,et al.  Reinforcement learning can account for associative and perceptual learning on a visual decision task , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. , 2010, Psychological review.

[31]  J. Gold,et al.  Caudate Encodes Multiple Computations for Perceptual Decisions , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Timothy J. Pleskac,et al.  Two-stage dynamic signal detection: a theory of choice, decision time, and confidence. , 2010, Psychological review.

[33]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Neuronal basis of sequential foraging decisions in a patchy environment , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  John M. Pearson,et al.  Posterior cingulate cortex: adapting behavior to a changing world , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  Neural correlates of reliability-based cue weighting during multisensory integration , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Paul G. Middlebrooks,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Metacognition in Primate Frontal Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[37]  L. Maloney,et al.  Comparison of the Distortion of Probability Information in Decision Under Risk and an Equivalent Visual Task , 2012, Psychological science.

[38]  Mattias P. Karlsson,et al.  Network Resets in Medial Prefrontal Cortex Mark the Onset of Behavioral Uncertainty , 2012, Science.

[39]  Marius Usher,et al.  Using Time-Varying Evidence to Test Models of Decision Dynamics: Bounded Diffusion vs. the Leaky Competing Accumulator Model , 2012, Front. Neurosci..

[40]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  Striatal activations signal prediction errors on confidence in the absence of external feedback , 2012, NeuroImage.

[41]  Ariel Zylberberg,et al.  The construction of confidence in a perceptual decision , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[42]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Rational regulation of learning dynamics by pupil–linked arousal systems , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[43]  Marius Usher,et al.  The Timescale of Perceptual Evidence Integration Can Be Adapted to the Environment , 2013, Current Biology.

[44]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Decision Making as a Window on Cognition , 2013, Neuron.

[45]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Modeling confidence judgments, response times, and multiple choices in decision making: recognition memory and motion discrimination. , 2013, Psychological review.

[46]  Etienne Koechlin,et al.  Foundations of human reasoning in the prefrontal cortex , 2014, Science.

[47]  Alexandre Pouget,et al.  Relation between Belief and Performance in Perceptual Decision Making , 2014, PloS one.

[48]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time , 2014, Neuron.

[49]  C. H. Donahue,et al.  Neural correlates of strategic reasoning during competitive games , 2014, Science.

[50]  H. Lau,et al.  How to measure metacognition , 2014, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[51]  M. Bezanilla,et al.  Myosin VIII associates with microtubule ends and together with actin plays a role in guiding plant cell division , 2014, eLife.

[52]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The Perception of Probability , 2022 .

[53]  Roozbeh Kiani,et al.  A neural mechanism of speed-accuracy tradeoff in macaque area LIP , 2014, eLife.

[54]  Bingni W. Brunton,et al.  Distinct relationships of parietal and prefrontal cortices to evidence accumulation , 2014, Nature.

[55]  M. Usher,et al.  Post choice information integration as a causal determinant of confidence: Novel data and a computational account , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[56]  Florent Meyniel,et al.  The Sense of Confidence during Probabilistic Learning: A Normative Account , 2015, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[57]  M. Shadlen,et al.  A Neural Implementation of Wald’s Sequential Probability Ratio Test , 2015, Neuron.

[58]  Joseph W Kable,et al.  Normative evidence accumulation in unpredictable environments , 2015, eLife.

[59]  Timothy J. Pleskac,et al.  Dynamics of postdecisional processing of confidence. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[60]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Precision and neuronal dynamics in the human posterior parietal cortex during evidence accumulation , 2015, NeuroImage.

[61]  Vincent D Costa,et al.  Reversal Learning and Dopamine: A Bayesian Perspective , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  Roozbeh Kiani,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Post-error Adjustments of Decision Policy in Parietal Cortex , 2016, Neuron.

[63]  P. Sterzer,et al.  Mesolimbic confidence signals guide perceptual learning in the absence of external feedback , 2016, eLife.