Sequence effects support the conflict theory of N2 and P3 in the Go/NoGo task.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Andrew Heathcote | A. Heathcote | Janette L. Smith | Elizabeth A Smith | A. Provost | Janette L Smith | Alexander L Provost
[1] T. Braver,et al. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.
[2] H. Semlitsch,et al. A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[3] M. W. Molen,et al. A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.
[4] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a go/no-go task: Effects of response conflict and trial type frequency , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[5] H. Garavan,et al. Dissociable Executive Functions in the Dynamic Control of Behavior: Inhibition, Error Detection, and Correction , 2002, NeuroImage.
[6] Robert J. Barry,et al. The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: Indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.
[7] M K Kaiser,et al. MANOVA method for analyzing repeated measures designs: an extensive primer. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.
[8] J. Pekar,et al. fMRI evidence that the neural basis of response inhibition is task-dependent. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[9] Dorret I. Boomsma,et al. Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: Conflict adaptation or associative priming? , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[10] B. J. Casey,et al. The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.
[11] H. Bokura,et al. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[12] I. Jentzsch,et al. Functional localization and mechanisms of sequential effects in serial reaction time tasks , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[13] S. Mäntysalo,et al. N2 and P3 of the ERP to Go and Nogo stimuli: a stimulus-response association and dissociation. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.
[14] R. Barry,et al. Movement-related potentials in the Go/NoGo task: The P3 reflects both cognitive and motor inhibition , 2008, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[15] Yihong Yang,et al. A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control , 2002 .
[16] M. Jarvik,et al. Probability learning and a negative recency effect in the serial anticipation of alternative symbols. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.
[17] Carolin Dudschig,et al. Locus of response slowing resulting from alternation-based processing interference. , 2008, Psychophysiology.
[18] Jacqueline A. Rushby,et al. An orienting reflex perspective on anteriorisation of the P3 of the event-related potential , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[19] E. Soetens. Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure , 1998 .
[20] J Horváth,et al. Simultaneously active pre-attentive representations of local and global rules for sound sequences in the human brain. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[21] R. J. Doherty,et al. Separation of the components of the late positive complex in an ERP dishabituation paradigm , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[22] E. Bullmore,et al. Mapping Motor Inhibition: Conjunctive Brain Activations across Different Versions of Go/No-Go and Stop Tasks , 2001, NeuroImage.
[23] A. A. Wijers,et al. Inhibition, response mode, and stimulus probability: a comparative event-related potential study , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[24] J. Kenemans,et al. The pure electrophysiology of stopping. , 2005, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[25] I. Jentzsch,et al. Response conflict determines sequential effects in serial response time tasks with short response-stimulus intervals. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] Hisae Gemba,et al. Potential related to no-go reaction of go/no-go hand movement task with color discrimination in human , 1989, Neuroscience Letters.
[28] A Pfefferbaum,et al. ERPs to stimuli requiring response production and inhibition: effects of age, probability and visual noise. , 1988, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[29] W. Sommer,et al. Consciousness of attention and expectancy as reflected in event-related potentials and reaction times. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[30] P. Bertelson. Sequential Redundancy and Speed in a Serial Two-Choice Responding Task , 1961 .
[31] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[32] R. Ratcliff,et al. A model of the go/no-go task. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[33] K. J. Bruin,et al. Response priming in a go/nogo task: do we have to explain the go/nogo N2 effect in terms of response activation instead of inhibition? , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[34] A. Kok. Effects of degradation of visual stimuli on components of the event-related potential (ERP) in go/nogo reaction tasks , 1986, Biological Psychology.
[35] H Pratt,et al. Ear advantage and attention: an ERP study of auditory cued attention , 2004, Hearing Research.
[36] Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition , 2004, Brain and Cognition.
[37] Robert J. Barry,et al. Response priming in the Go/NoGo task: The N2 reflects neither inhibition nor conflict , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[38] Antonino Vallesi,et al. FMRI evidence of a functional network setting the criteria for withholding a response , 2009, NeuroImage.
[39] J. Kenemans,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of attention, inhibition, sensitivity and bias in a continuous performance task , 2004, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[40] E. Donchin,et al. Probability effects on stimulus evaluation and response processes. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] W. Sommer,et al. Differential effects of voluntary expectancies on reaction times and event-related potentials: evidence for automatic and controlled expectancies. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[42] J. Polich,et al. P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[43] C. Carter,et al. The Timing of Action-Monitoring Processes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[44] B. Tabachnick,et al. Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .
[45] Evian Gordon,et al. Numbers of preceding nontargets differentially affect responses to targets in normal volunteers and patients with schizophrenia: A study of event-related potentials , 1995, Psychiatry Research.
[46] Yuping Wang,et al. Event-Related Potentials in a No-Go Task Involving Response-Tendency Conflict , 2002, Clinical EEG.
[47] E. Soetens,et al. Covert signs of expectancy in serial reaction time tasks revealed by event-related potentials , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[48] Frederick Verbruggen,et al. Short-term aftereffects of response inhibition: repetition priming or between-trial control adjustments? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[49] Erwin Hennighausen,et al. N200 in the Eriksen-Task: Inhibitory Executive Processes? , 2000 .
[50] Margot J. Taylor,et al. Guidelines for using human event-related potentials to study cognition: recording standards and publication criteria. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[51] Robert J. Remington,et al. Analysis of Sequential Effects for a Four-Choice Reaction Time Experiment. , 1971 .
[52] E. Golob,et al. Effects of stimulus sequence on event-related potentials and reaction time during target detection in Alzheimer's disease , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[53] Kevin Murphy,et al. Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory control , 2006, Brain Research.
[54] Donald Laming,et al. Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .
[55] N. Squires,et al. The effect of stimulus sequence on the waveform of the cortical event-related potential. , 1976, Science.
[56] P Sandroni,et al. Readiness to respond in a target detection task: pre- and post-stimulus event-related potentials in normal subjects. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[57] K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[58] A. Nativ,et al. Potentials associated with the initiation and inhibition of visually triggered finger movement in humans: the "no-go potential" in the go/no-go paradigm. , 1992, International Journal of Neuroscience.
[59] I. Jentzsch,et al. Sequence-sensitive subcomponents of P300: topographical analyses and dipole source localization. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[60] N. Kirby. Sequential effects in two-choice reaction time: automatic facilitation or subjective expectancy? , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] E. Jodo,et al. Relation of a negative ERP component to response inhibition in a Go/No-go task. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[62] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[63] I. Winkler. Interpreting the Mismatch Negativity , 2007 .
[64] E Donchin,et al. P300 and stimulus categorization: two plus one is not so different from one plus one. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[65] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The effects of stimulus sequence on event related potentials: A comparison of visual and auditory sequences , 1977 .
[66] Hiroshi Fukuda,et al. The human prefrontal and parietal association cortices are involved in NO-GO performances—an event-related fMRI study , 2000, NeuroImage.
[67] Jonathan R. Folstein,et al. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. , 2007, Psychophysiology.
[68] B. Kopp,et al. N2, P3 and the lateralized readiness potential in a nogo task involving selective response priming. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[69] R. Barry,et al. Effects of pre-stimulus processing on subsequent events in a warned Go/NoGo paradigm: response preparation, execution and inhibition. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[70] R. Näätänen,et al. Sequential effects on the ERP in discriminating two stimuli , 1983, Biological Psychology.
[71] R. Remington. Analysis of sequential effects in choice reaction times. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[72] G. Band,et al. Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.