Fear-potentiated startle responses in temperamentally different human infants.

Previous research has indicated that 4-month-old human infants who exhibit high degrees of motor activity and negative affect in response to the presentation of unfamiliar auditory and visual stimuli are likely to display behavioral inhibition as toddlers, while 4-month-old infants who display high degrees of motor activity and positive affect in response to the same stimuli are likely to be behaviorally exuberant toddlers. The present study examined baseline and fear-potentiated startle eyeblink responses during a stranger-approach paradigm at age 9 months in a group of infants, some of whom displayed high motor activity and negative affect and some of whom displayed high motor activity and positive affect at 4 months. The analyses revealed that the high motor/high negative group of infants exhibited a significantly greater increase in fear-potentiated startle amplitude at 9 months compared with the high motor/high positive group. There were no differences among groups of infants on baseline startle responses. These findings suggest that the origins of behavioral inhibition in early childhood may be linked to a low threshold for arousal in forebrain limbic areas.

[1]  N. Fox,et al.  Behavioral and physiological antecedents of inhibited and uninhibited behavior. , 1996, Child development.

[2]  M. Bradley,et al.  Startle and emotion: lateral acoustic probes and the bilateral blink. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[3]  E. Cook,et al.  Affective individual differences and startle reflex modulation. , 1991, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[4]  M. Balaban,et al.  Affective influences on startle in five-month-old infants: reactions to facial expressions of emotions. , 1995, Child development.

[5]  N. Fox,et al.  If it's not left, it's right. Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the development of emotion. , 1991, The American psychologist.

[6]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  J. Kagan,et al.  Stable behavioral inhibition and its association with anxiety disorder. , 1992, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[8]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Frontal activation asymmetry and social competence at four years of age. , 1995, Child development.

[9]  N. Frijda,et al.  Modulation of the acoustic startle response by film-induced fear and sexual arousal. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[10]  M. Gallagher,et al.  The amygdala complex: multiple roles in associative learning and attention. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  N. Fox,et al.  Acoustic startle electromyographic (EMG) activity indexed from an electroculographic (EOG) electrode placement: a methodological note. , 1998, The International journal of neuroscience.

[12]  F. Graham,et al.  Prestimulation effects on blink and cardiac reflexes of 15-month human infants. , 1989, Developmental psychobiology.

[13]  Temperamental factors in human development. , 1991 .

[14]  M. Bradley,et al.  Startle reflex modification: emotion or attention? , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[15]  Michela Gallagher,et al.  Amygdala central nucleus lesions: Effect on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit , 1979, Physiology & Behavior.

[16]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Interruption of projections from the medial geniculate body to an archi-neostriatal field disrupts the classical conditioning of emotional responses to acoustic stimuli , 1986, Neuroscience.

[17]  P. Lang,et al.  The startle probe response: a new measure of emotion? , 1988, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  Michael Davis,et al.  Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[19]  Kenneth H. Rubin,et al.  Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. , 1997, Developmental psychobiology.

[20]  C. Braun,et al.  Emotional qualities of odors and their influence on the startle reflex in humans. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Peter J. Lang,et al.  Emotion, novelty, and the startle reflex: habituation in humans. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[22]  Kagan,et al.  The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. , 1987, Child development.

[23]  H. Schnitzler,et al.  Acoustic startle response and habituation in freezing and nonfreezing rats. , 1993, Behavioral neuroscience.

[24]  J. Schulkin,et al.  Hyperexcitability: exaggerated fear-potentiated startle produced by partial amygdala kindling. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  Infants' reactions to an approaching stranger: description, validation, and functional significance of wariness. , 1975 .

[26]  J. Kagan,et al.  Biological bases of childhood shyness. , 1988, Science.

[27]  M. Bradley,et al.  Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. , 1990, Psychological review.

[28]  C. Grillon,et al.  Fear-potentiated startle: Relationship to the level of state/trait anxiety in healthy subjects , 1993, Biological Psychiatry.

[29]  Michael Davis,et al.  Lesions of the amygdala, but not of the cerebellum or red nucleus, block conditioned fear as measured with the potentiated startle paradigm. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[30]  M. Gallagher,et al.  The amygdala central nucleus and appetitive Pavlovian conditioning: lesions impair one class of conditioned behavior , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  M. Davis,et al.  Enhancement of acoustic startle by electrical stimulation of the amygdala. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.