Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans.

Because of attentional limitations, the human visual system can process for awareness and response only a fraction of the input received. Lesion and functional imaging studies have identified frontal, temporal, and parietal areas as playing a major role in the attentional control of visual processing, but very little is known about how these areas interact to form a dynamic attentional network. We hypothesized that the network communicates by means of neural phase synchronization, and we used magnetoencephalography to study transient long-range interarea phase coupling in a well studied attentionally taxing dual-target task (attentional blink). Our results reveal that communication within the fronto-parieto-temporal attentional network proceeds via transient long-range phase synchronization in the beta band. Changes in synchronization reflect changes in the attentional demands of the task and are directly related to behavioral performance. Thus, we show how attentional limitations arise from the way in which the subsystems of the attentional network interact.

[1]  W. Singer,et al.  Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time-lag synchronization among cortical areas , 1997, Nature.

[2]  Wolf Singer,et al.  Neuronal Synchrony: A Versatile Code for the Definition of Relations? , 1999, Neuron.

[3]  K L Shapiro,et al.  Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Functional localization of the system for visuospatial attention using positron emission tomography. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  E. Vogel,et al.  Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink , 1996, Nature.

[6]  Wolf Singer,et al.  Time as coding space? , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  W. Singer,et al.  Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation , 1994 .

[9]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision , 1994, Nature.

[10]  O. Bertrand,et al.  Oscillatory Synchrony between Human Extrastriate Areas during Visual Short-Term Memory Maintenance , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  D. P. Russell,et al.  Increased Synchronization of Neuromagnetic Responses during Conscious Perception , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Henson,et al.  Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  Anil K. Seth,et al.  The power of human brain magnetoencephalographic signals can be modulated up or down by changes in an attentive visual task , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[15]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  A. Schnitzler,et al.  The neural basis of intermittent motor control in humans , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  E. Niebur,et al.  Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps , 2022 .

[18]  S. Bressler,et al.  Synchronized activity in prefrontal cortex during anticipation of visuomotor processing , 2002, Neuroreport.

[19]  F. Varela,et al.  Perception's shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity , 1999, Nature.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex and the filtering of distractors , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  James T. Enns,et al.  Visual masking plays two roles in the attentional blink , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  Christoph von der Malsburg,et al.  The What and Why of Binding The Modeler’s Perspective , 1999, Neuron.

[23]  G. Ermentrout,et al.  Gamma rhythms and beta rhythms have different synchronization properties. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[24]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Roger D. Traub,et al.  Long-Range Synchronization of γ and β Oscillations and the Plasticity of Excitatory and Inhibitory Synapses: A Network Model , 2002 .

[26]  A. Wróbel,et al.  Beta activity: a carrier for visual attention. , 2000, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[27]  V. Lollo,et al.  Beyond the attentional blink: visual masking by object substitution. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[29]  J Gross,et al.  REPRINTS , 1962, The Lancet.

[30]  Wolf Singer,et al.  Neurobiology: Striving for coherence , 1999, Nature.

[31]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[32]  M. Hallett,et al.  Transient Interhemispheric Neuronal Synchrony Correlates with Object Recognition , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Eckhorn,et al.  Perception-related modulations of local field potential power and coherence in primary visual cortex of awake monkey during binocular rivalry. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  J. Martinerie,et al.  The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[35]  P. König,et al.  Top-down processing mediated by interareal synchronization. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  J. Gore,et al.  Neural Correlates of the Attentional Blink , 2000, Neuron.

[37]  W. Singer,et al.  Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  H Petsche,et al.  Synchronization between temporal and parietal cortex during multimodal object processing in man. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  H. Petsche,et al.  Synchronization between prefrontal and posterior association cortex during human working memory. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  Christoph Braun,et al.  Coherence of gamma-band EEG activity as a basis for associative learning , 1999, Nature.

[42]  M. Hallett,et al.  Integrative visuomotor behavior is associated with interregionally coherent oscillations in the human brain. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  J. Fuster,et al.  From perception to action: temporal integrative functions of prefrontal and parietal neurons. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.