Processing of Japanese morphogram and syllabogram in the left basal temporal area: electrical cortical stimulation studies.

Language functions in the left basal temporal area (LBTA) were investigated using electrical cortical stimulation during functional mapping in six Japanese patients with refractory epilepsy. This study provides the first direct evidence that kana (Japanese syllabogram) is processed in the LBTA. Electrical stimulation of some areas within LBTA induced disturbance in overt reading of kana words only in the first trials, with no errors in the subsequent trials. By contrast, stimulation of the same area caused obvious disturbance in kana non-word reading in all trials. Since a kana word carries both meaning and sound while a kana non-word carries only sounds of a letter string, the contrasting results of partial and complete disturbance imply a possibility that there are two distinct pathways for kana reading: one dealing with both phonological and semantic aspects of the words and the other dealing only with phonological aspect. Kanji words (Japanese morphogram) and objects/pictures were found to be processed in an area different from the area for the kana non-word processing. Furthermore, the present study also identified the common area for processing kanji reading and object/picture naming. There were no errors in matching pictures with kanji words, indicating that concepts of pictures and meanings of kanji words were not interfered by the electrical stimulation of that area. The new insight provides a clue for partial description of processing pathways for language-related visual information in LBTA. Three types of information (morphological, phonological, and semantic) are conveyed together at some stages and are separated into different routes at some other stages.

[1]  D L Miglioretti,et al.  Temporal dynamics of verbal object comprehension. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  I. Kanazawa,et al.  Different cortical activity in reading of Kanji words, Kana words and Kana nonwords. , 2000, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[4]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  S. Kuriki,et al.  Neural processing of words in the human extrastriate visual cortex. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  The Neural Substrate of Picture Naming , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  H Chertkow,et al.  Dissociable brain regions process object meaning and object structure during picture naming , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  R P Lesser,et al.  Cognitive Effects of Resecting Basal Temporal Language Areas , 1996, Epilepsia.

[10]  H. Lüders,et al.  Basal temporal language area. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  S. Bookheimer,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow during object naming and word reading , 1995 .

[12]  M. Farah,et al.  Neural Specialization for Letter Recognition , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  Hiroshi Shibasaki,et al.  Conversion of semantic information into phonological representation: a function in left posterior basal temporal area. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[14]  J B Poline,et al.  Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  R. Salmelin,et al.  Dynamics of visual feature analysis and object-level processing in face versus letter-string perception. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  A. Yamadori,et al.  Pure Kana Agraphia as A Manifestation of Graphemic Buffer Impairment , 2001, Cortex.

[17]  H. Lüders,et al.  Movement-related potentials recorded from supplementary motor area and primary motor area. Role of supplementary motor area in voluntary movements. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  S Lehéricy,et al.  The visual word form area: spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[19]  H. Lüders,et al.  Quantitative Comparison of Language Deficits Produced by Extraoperative Electrical Stimulation of Broca's, Wernicke's, and Basal Temporal Language Areas , 1996, Epilepsia.

[20]  R P Lesser,et al.  Basal temporal language area demonstrated by electrical stimulation , 1986, Neurology.

[21]  S. Petersen,et al.  A procedure for identifying regions preferentially activated by attention to semantic and phonological relations using functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Joel R. Meyer,et al.  Modality independence of word comprehension , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[23]  M. Sugishita,et al.  Lexical agraphia in the Japanese language. Pure agraphia for Kanji due to left posteroinferior temporal lesions. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  Makoto Iwata,et al.  Kanji versus Kananeuropsychological correlates of the Japanese writing system , 1984, Trends in Neurosciences.

[25]  C. Fiebach,et al.  fMRI Evidence for Dual Routes to the Mental Lexicon in Visual Word Recognition , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  C. Price,et al.  Three Distinct Ventral Occipitotemporal Regions for Reading and Object Naming , 1999, NeuroImage.

[27]  R P Lesser,et al.  Characterization of the basal temporal language area in patients with left temporal lobe epilepsy , 1990, Neurology.

[28]  W H Theodore,et al.  Cortical Stimulation Elicits Regional Distinctions in Auditory and Visual Naming , 1996, Epilepsia.

[29]  M. Honda,et al.  Participation of the left posterior inferior temporal cortex in writing and mental recall of kanji orthography: A functional MRI study. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.