When SOFA primes TOUCH: Interdependence of spelling, sound, and meaning in “semantically mediated” phonological priming

Three experiments test for semantically mediated priming of a word’s phonology (e.g.,sofa, an associate of couch, primes naming performance totouch). In the first two experiments, words that were body-rime-inconsistent (comparetouch tocouch) were used as naming targets. In the third experiment, words that were body-rime-consistent were also used (i.e.,sofa primedpouch). Low-frequency inconsistent words yield a high rate of pronunciation errors when they were primed byindirectly related words, such assofa, in both a standard naming task and a speeded naming task. High-frequency inconsistent words yielded slower naming times when they were primed by indirectly related words in a speeded naming task, but consistent words showed no significant effects of the primes. The results suggest that the relationship between semantics and phonology plays an important, early role in word perception.

[1]  Alan H. Kawamoto,et al.  Pronunciation of homographs , 1992 .

[2]  Lucia Colombo,et al.  Inhibitory Effects in Form Priming: Evaluating a Phonological Competition Explanation , 1994 .

[3]  M. Turvey,et al.  Visual lexical access is initially phonological: 1. Evidence from associative priming by words, homophones, and pseudohomophones. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[4]  L. Colombo Activation and inhibition with orthographically similar words. , 1986 .

[5]  R. Glushko The Organization and Activation of Orthographic Knowledge in Reading Aloud. , 1979 .

[6]  A. H. Kawamoto Nonlinear dynamics in the resolution of lexical ambiguity: A parallel distributed processing account. , 1993 .

[7]  G. C. Orden,et al.  Interdependence of form and function in cognitive systems explains perception of printed words. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  Georgije Lukatela,et al.  Phonemic similarity effects and prelexical phonology , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[9]  A Pollatsek,et al.  Phonological codes and eye movements in reading. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  Gary S. Dell,et al.  The retrieval of phonological forms in production: tests of predictions from a connectionist model , 1988 .

[11]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. , 1996, Psychological review.

[12]  M Coltheart,et al.  Reading aloud begins when the computation of phonology is complete. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  William T. IV Farrar,et al.  Errors as Multistable Response Options , 2001 .

[14]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Does Word Identification Proceed From Spelling to Sound to Meaning , 1991 .

[15]  G S Dell,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. , 1986, Psychological review.

[16]  M. Rosson From SOFA to LOUCH: Lexical contributions to pseudoword pronunciation , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[17]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  Debra Jared,et al.  Spelling-Sound Consistency Affects the Naming of High-Frequency Words , 1997 .

[19]  W. T. Farrar,et al.  Investigating single-word syntactic primes in naming tasks: a recurrent network approach. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  G. C. Orden,et al.  Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[21]  G. Dell,et al.  Mediated and convergent lexical priming in language production: a comment on Levelt et al. (1991). , 1991, Psychological review.

[22]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  The basis of consistency effects in word naming , 1990 .

[23]  M. Daneman,et al.  Phonological recoding in silent reading , 1991 .

[24]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Semantic effects in single-word naming. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[25]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. , 1987 .

[26]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. , 1989, Psychological review.

[27]  D. Mackay The Organization of Perception and Action , 1987 .

[28]  M T Turvey,et al.  Reading in two alphabets. , 1998, The American psychologist.

[29]  A. Pollatsek,et al.  Automatic access of semantic information by phonological codes in visual word recognition. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  Dan Chateau,et al.  Exposure to print and word recognition processes , 2000, Memory & cognition.

[31]  Padraig G. O'Seaghdha,et al.  Mediated Semantic-Phonological Priming: Calling Distant Relatives , 1997 .

[32]  M. Masson A distributed memory model of semantic priming. , 1995 .

[33]  G C Van Orden,et al.  Pathway selection's utility for control of word recognition. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  P. O'Seaghdha,et al.  Phonological competition and cooperation in form-related priming: sequential and nonsequential processes in word production. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.