The affective consequences of cognitive inhibition: devaluation or neutralization?

Affective evaluations of previously ignored visual stimuli are more negative than those of novel items or prior targets of attention or response. This has been taken as evidence that inhibition has negative affective consequences. But inhibition could act instead to attenuate or "neutralize" preexisting affective salience, predicting opposite effects for stimuli that were initially positive or negative in valence. We tested this hypothesis by presenting trustworthy and untrustworthy faces (Experiment 1), strongly positive and negative photographs (Experiment 2), and monetary gain- and loss-associated patterns (Experiment 3) in a Go/No-Go task and assessing subsequent affective ratings. Evaluations of prior No-Go (inhibited) stimuli were more negative than of prior Go (noninhibited) stimuli, regardless of a priori affective valence. Ratings of No-Go stimuli also became increasingly negative (vs. increasingly neutral) when preexisting salience was increased via stimulus repetition (Experiment 4). Our results suggest inhibition leads to affective devaluation, not affective neutralization.

[1]  Harm Veling,et al.  When approach motivation and behavioral inhibition collide: Behavior regulation through stimulus devaluation , 2018 .

[2]  Klaus Kessler,et al.  Attentional Inhibition Has Social-Emotional Consequences for Unfamiliar Faces , 2005, Psychological science.

[3]  D. Schacter,et al.  Priming and the Brain , 1998, Neuron.

[4]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Human Brain Function , 1997 .

[5]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  Modeling Visual Attention via Selective Tuning , 1995, Artif. Intell..

[6]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[7]  George Adelman,et al.  Encyclopedia of neuroscience , 2004 .

[8]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Affective Influences of Selective Attention , 2006 .

[9]  E Fox,et al.  Interference and negative priming from ignored distractors: The role of selection difficulty , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  R. Zajonc Mere Exposure: A Gateway to the Subliminal , 2001 .

[11]  John K. Tsotsos,et al.  An Attentional Mechanism for Selecting Appropriate Actions Afforded by Graspable Objects , 2008, Psychological science.

[12]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[13]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Long-term gaze cueing effects: Evidence for retrieval of prior states of attention from memory , 2006 .

[14]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[15]  Jane E Raymond,et al.  Emotional devaluation of distracting patterns and faces: a consequence of attentional inhibition during visual search? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[17]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[18]  John G. Taylor,et al.  Efficient Attentional Selection Predicts Distractor Devaluation: Event-related Potential Evidence for a Direct Link between Attention and Emotion , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Brian A. Goolsby,et al.  Feature-based inhibition underlies the affective consequences of attention , 2009 .

[20]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .

[21]  Russell A Poldrack,et al.  Cognition: Basal Ganglia Role , 2009 .

[22]  S. Tipper,et al.  Long-Term Inhibition of Return of Attention , 2003, Psychological science.

[23]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  D. Zald The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[25]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  D. Tucker,et al.  Electrophysiological Responses to Errors and Feedback in the Process of Action Regulation , 2003, Psychological science.

[27]  J. Desmond,et al.  Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  M. Fenske,et al.  Modulation of focused attention by faces expressing emotion: evidence from flanker tasks. , 2003, Emotion.

[29]  S. Tipper,et al.  Long-Term Negative Priming: Support for Retrieval of Prior Attentional Processes , 2005, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[30]  R. Henson,et al.  Neural response suppression, haemodynamic repetition effects, and behavioural priming , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[31]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Dissociable prefrontal brain systems for attention and emotion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Melina A. Kunar,et al.  The affective consequences of visual attention in preview search , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Different neural systems adjust motor behavior in response to reward and punishment , 2007, NeuroImage.

[34]  P. Holland,et al.  Amygdala–frontal interactions and reward expectancy , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  Brian A. Goolsby,et al.  Distractor devaluation requires visual working memory , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[36]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Emotional Attention , 2009 .

[37]  M. Beauregard,et al.  Neural Correlates of Conscious Self-Regulation of Emotion , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  A. Nobre,et al.  Response Inhibition is Linked to Emotional Devaluation: Behavioural and Electrophysiological Evidence , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[39]  Tamara C. Cristescu,et al.  Modelling distractor devaluation (DD) and its neurophysiological correlates , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  K. Phan,et al.  Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[41]  D L Strayer,et al.  Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than what meets the eye , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[43]  D. Strayer,et al.  Negative identity priming is contingent on stimulus repetition. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  A. Martínez,et al.  The AR face databasae , 1998 .

[45]  R. Ward,et al.  Selective attention and control of action: comparative psychology of an artificial, evolved agent and people. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[46]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  J. Schwarzbach,et al.  Different time courses for visual perception and action priming , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[48]  N Lavie,et al.  The role of perceptual load in negative priming. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  R. Davidson,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  P. Lang International affective picture system (IAPS) : affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual , 2005 .

[51]  Jane E Raymond,et al.  Selective Attention Determines Emotional Responses to Novel Visual Stimuli , 2003, Psychological science.

[52]  Francisco Esteves,et al.  The Face of Wrath: Critical Features for Conveying Facial Threat , 1999 .

[53]  L. F. Barrett Discrete Emotions or Dimensions? The Role of Valence Focus and Arousal Focus , 1998 .

[54]  A. Treisman,et al.  Visual memory for novel shapes: implicit coding without attention. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[55]  Aleix M. Martinez,et al.  The AR face database , 1998 .

[56]  D. Pandya,et al.  Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[57]  S. Tipper,et al.  Behavioural Goals Determine Inhibitory Mechanisms of Selective Attention , 1994 .

[58]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The rise and fall of priming: how visual exposure shapes cortical representations of objects. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  Oren Griffiths,et al.  Negative priming reduces affective ratings , 2008 .

[60]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[61]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Reciprocal links between emotion and attention , 2003 .