Stimulus context modulates competition in human extrastriate cortex

When multiple stimuli appear simultaneously in the visual field, they are not processed independently, but rather interact in a mutually suppressive way, suggesting that they compete for neural representation in visual cortex. The biased competition model of selective attention predicts that the competition can be influenced by both top-down and bottom-up mechanisms. Directed attention has been shown to bias competition in favor of the attended stimulus in extrastriate cortex. Here, we show that suppressive interactions among multiple stimuli are eliminated in extrastriate cortex when they are presented in the context of pop-out displays, in which a single item differs from the others, but not in heterogeneous displays, in which all items differ from each other. The pop-out effects seemed to originate in early visual cortex and were independent of attentional top-down control, suggesting that stimulus context may provide a powerful influence on neural competition in human visual cortex.

[1]  D. Broadbent Perception and communication , 1958 .

[2]  William Prinzmetal,et al.  Configurational effects in visual information processing , 1976 .

[3]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  C Bundesen,et al.  Color segregation and visual search , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  J. Duncan Selective attention and the organization of visual information. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[7]  S Ullman,et al.  Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. , 1985, Human neurobiology.

[8]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[9]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[10]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  G. Baylis,et al.  Movement and visual attention: the spotlight metaphor breaks down. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[12]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.

[13]  Jeremy M Wolfe,et al.  Modeling the role of parallel processing in visual search , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  The role of the primate extrastriate area V4 in vision. , 1991, Science.

[15]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  I. Rock,et al.  Perceptual organization and attention , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  Jon Driver,et al.  Preserved figure-ground segregation and symmetry perception in visual neglect , 1992, Nature.

[18]  E. Miller,et al.  Suppression of visual responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex of the awake macaque by addition of a second stimulus , 1993, Brain Research.

[19]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  MRI‐PET Registration with Automated Algorithm , 1993, Journal of computer assisted tomography.

[20]  H. Nothdurft The role of features in preattentive vision: Comparison of orientation, motion and color cues , 1993, Vision Research.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited , 1995, NeuroImage.

[22]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[23]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[24]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[25]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. , 1998, Science.

[27]  J. Duncan Converging levels of analysis in the cognitive neuroscience of visual attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[28]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[30]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Response modulation by texture surround in primate area V1: Correlates of “popout” under anesthesia , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[31]  S. Kastner,et al.  Neuronal responses to orientation and motion contrast in cat striate cortex , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[32]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Microsaccadic eye movements and firing of single cells in the striate cortex of macaque monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[33]  S. Yantis,et al.  On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Z Li,et al.  Contextual influences in V1 as a basis for pop out and asymmetry in visual search. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  G H Recanzone,et al.  Effects of attention on MT and MST neuronal activity during pursuit initiation. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Erratum to “Translocation machinery for synthesis of integral membrane and secretory proteins in dendritic spines” , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  S. Yantis 3 Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven Determinants of Attentional Control , 2000 .

[38]  J. Gallant,et al.  A Human Extrastriate Area Functionally Homologous to Macaque V4 , 2000, Neuron.

[39]  C. Koch,et al.  Computational modelling of visual attention , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[40]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Modulation of sensory suppression: implications for receptive field sizes in the human visual cortex. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  R. Desimone,et al.  Interacting Roles of Attention and Visual Salience in V4 , 2003, Neuron.

[44]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Hidehiko Komatsu,et al.  Target Selection in Area V4 during a Multidimensional Visual Search Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Attention alters appearance , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Frith,et al.  Neural Correlates of Attentional Capture in Visual Search , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  M. A. Steinmetz,et al.  Posterior Parietal Cortex Automatically Encodes the Location of Salient Stimuli , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.