Lower Parietal Encoding Activation Is Associated with Sharper Information and Better Memory

Abstract Mean fMRI activation in ventral posterior parietal cortex (vPPC) during memory encoding often negatively predicts successful remembering. A popular interpretation of this phenomenon is that vPPC reflects “off‐task” processing. However, recent fMRI studies considering distributed patterns of activity suggest that vPPC actively represents encoded material. Here, we assessed the relationships between pattern‐based content representations in vPPC, mean activation in vPPC, and subsequent remembering. We analyzed data from two fMRI experiments where subjects studied then recalled word‐face or word‐scene associations. For each encoding trial, we measured 1) mean univariate activation within vPPC and 2) the strength of face/scene information as indexed by pattern analysis. Mean activation in vPPC negatively predicted subsequent remembering, but the strength of pattern‐based information in the same vPPC voxels positively predicted later memory. Indeed, univariate amplitude averaged across vPPC voxels negatively correlated with pattern‐based information strength. This dissociation reflected a tendency for univariate reductions to maximally occur in voxels that were not strongly tuned for the category of encoded stimuli. These results indicate that vPPC activity patterns reflect the content and quality of memory encoding and constitute a striking example of lower univariate activity corresponding to stronger pattern‐based information.

[1]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  J. Desmond,et al.  Making memories: brain activity that predicts how well visual experience will be remembered. , 1998, Science.

[4]  A. Dale,et al.  Building memories: remembering and forgetting of verbal experiences as predicted by brain activity. , 1998, Science.

[5]  L. Davachi,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Forgetting of things past , 2001, Current Biology.

[6]  R. Buckner,et al.  THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF REMEMBERING , 2001 .

[7]  I. Tendolkar,et al.  Integrated brain activity in medial temporal and prefrontal areas predicts subsequent memory performance: human declarative memory formation at the system level , 2001, Brain Research Bulletin.

[8]  M. Rugg,et al.  When more means less neural activity related to unsuccessful memory encoding , 2001, Current Biology.

[9]  N. Tzourio-Mazoyer,et al.  Automated Anatomical Labeling of Activations in SPM Using a Macroscopic Anatomical Parcellation of the MNI MRI Single-Subject Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[10]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  K. Paller,et al.  Observing the transformation of experience into memory , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  S. M. Daselaar,et al.  When less means more: deactivations during encoding that predict subsequent memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[13]  Benjamin J. Shannon,et al.  Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  K. Grill-Spector,et al.  Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Sean M. Polyn,et al.  Beyond mind-reading: multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  M. Chun,et al.  Linking Implicit and Explicit Memory: Common Encoding Factors and Shared Representations , 2006, Neuron.

[17]  Lila Davachi,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Differential Encoding Mechanisms for Subsequent Associative Recognition and Free Recall , 2012 .

[18]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Right TPJ deactivation during visual search: functional significance and support for a filter hypothesis. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[20]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[21]  J. Simons,et al.  Parietal contributions to recollection: Electrophysiological evidence from aging and patients with parietal lesions , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[24]  Yadin Dudai,et al.  The posterior parietal cortex in recognition memory: A neuropsychological study , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  Kaia L. Vilberg,et al.  Functional significance of retrieval‐related activity in lateral parietal cortex: Evidence from fMRI and ERPs , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[27]  Melina R. Uncapher,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. , 2009, Learning & memory.

[28]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Posterior Midline and Ventral Parietal Activity is Associated with Retrieval Success and Encoding Failure , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[29]  Anthony D. Wagner,et al.  Posterior parietal cortex and episodic encoding: Insights from fMRI subsequent memory effects and dual-attention theory , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[30]  F. Tong,et al.  Decoding reveals the contents of visual working memory in early visual areas , 2009, Nature.

[31]  Sean M. Polyn,et al.  A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. , 2009, Psychological review.

[32]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  Dissociation Between Memory Accuracy and Memory Confidence Following Bilateral Parietal Lesions , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  Roberto Cabeza,et al.  Overlapping brain activity between episodic memory encoding and retrieval: Roles of the task-positive and task-negative networks , 2010, NeuroImage.

[34]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  The confounding effect of response amplitude on MVPA performance measures , 2011, NeuroImage.

[35]  Marisa O. Hollinshead,et al.  The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  R. Sperling,et al.  What goes down must come up: role of the posteromedial cortices in encoding and retrieval. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Fidelity of neural reactivation reveals competition between memories , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Hongkeun Kim,et al.  Neural activity that predicts subsequent memory and forgetting: A meta-analysis of 74 fMRI studies , 2011, NeuroImage.

[40]  Anthony D Wagner,et al.  Dissociable Effects of Top-Down and Bottom-Up Attention during Episodic Encoding , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Episodic retrieval and the cortical binding of relational activity , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[42]  Marian E. Berryhill,et al.  Insights from neuropsychology: pinpointing the role of the posterior parietal cortex in episodic and working memory , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[43]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[44]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Cognitive contributions of the ventral parietal cortex: an integrative theoretical account , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  S. M. Daselaar,et al.  Explaining the encoding/retrieval flip: Memory-related deactivations and activations in the posteromedial cortex , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Multi-voxel patterns of visual category representation during episodic encoding are predictive of subsequent memory , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Analyses of regional-average activation and multivoxel pattern information tell complementary stories , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  Frank Tong,et al.  Relationship between BOLD amplitude and pattern classification of orientation-selective activity in the human visual cortex , 2012, NeuroImage.

[49]  Marvin M. Chun,et al.  The Effect of Attention on Repetition Suppression and Multivoxel Pattern Similarity , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[50]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Interference with episodic memory retrieval following transcranial stimulation of the inferior but not the superior parietal lobule , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[51]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Repetition Suppression and Multi-Voxel Pattern Similarity Differentially Track Implicit and Explicit Visual Memory , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[52]  J. Mumford,et al.  Complementary role of frontoparietal activity and cortical pattern similarity in successful episodic memory encoding. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[53]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Dissociable Neural Mechanisms for Goal-Directed Versus Incidental Memory Reactivation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[54]  Keith A. Johnson,et al.  The ups and downs of the posteromedial cortex: age- and amyloid-related functional alterations of the encoding/retrieval flip in cognitively normal older adults. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Measuring neural representations with fMRI: practices and pitfalls , 2013, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[56]  M. Seghier The Angular Gyrus , 2013, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[57]  Nicole M. Long,et al.  Human intracranial high-frequency activity maps episodic memory formation in space and time , 2014, NeuroImage.

[58]  Zara M. Bergström,et al.  Continuous Theta Burst Stimulation of Angular Gyrus Reduces Subjective Recollection , 2014, PloS one.

[59]  Mark D'Esposito,et al.  Distributed and Dynamic Storage of Working Memory Stimulus Information in Extrastriate Cortex , 2014, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  Melina R. Uncapher,et al.  Functional heterogeneity in posterior parietal cortex across attention and episodic memory retrieval. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Successful Remembering Elicits Event-Specific Activity Patterns in Lateral Parietal Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[62]  J. Wixted,et al.  Impaired perception of mnemonic oldness, but not mnemonic newness, after parietal lobe damage , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  Jarrod A. Lewis-Peacock,et al.  Pruning of memories by context-based prediction error , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  K. Schleifer,et al.  Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks and associative memory , 2014 .

[65]  Adrian W. Gilmore,et al.  A parietal memory network revealed by multiple MRI methods , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[66]  C. Honey,et al.  Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[67]  Gina F. Humphreys,et al.  Fusion and Fission of Cognitive Functions in the Human Parietal Cortex , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[68]  Nachum Soroker,et al.  Parietal lesion effects on cued recall following pair associate learning , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  Hervé Abdi,et al.  Distributed Patterns of Reactivation Predict Vividness of Recollection , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  N. Turk-Browne,et al.  Attention Stabilizes Representations in the Human Hippocampus. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.