Robots Can Be More Than Black And White: Examining Racial Bias Towards Robots

Previous studies showed that using the 'shooter bias' paradigm, people demonstrate a similar racial bias toward dark colored robots over light colored robots (i.e., Black vs. White) as they do toward humans of similar skin tones [3]. However, such an effect could be argued to be the result of social priming. Additionally, it raises the question of how people might respond to robots that are in the middle of the color spectrum (i.e., brown) and whether such effects are moderated by the perceived anthropomorphism of the robots. We conducted two experiments to first examine whether shooter bias tendencies shown towards robots is driven by social priming, and then whether diversification of robot color and level of anthropomorphism influenced shooter bias. Our results showed that shooter bias was not influenced by social priming, and interestingly, introducing a new color of robot removed shooter bias tendencies entirely. However, varying the anthropomorphism of the robots did not moderate the level of shooter bias, and contrary to our expectations, the robots were not perceived by the participants as having different levels of anthropomorphism.

[1]  C. Judd,et al.  The police officer's dilemma: using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[2]  Ayanna M. Howard,et al.  Does Removing Stereotype Priming Remove Bias? A Pilot Human-Robot Interaction Study , 2018, ArXiv.

[3]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  When Fatigue Turns Deadly: The Association Between Fatigue and Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot , 2013 .

[4]  Konrad Bresin,et al.  Is the evidence from racial bias shooting task studies a smoking gun? Results from a meta-analysis , 2015 .

[5]  Kristina R. Olson,et al.  Exposure to Racial Out-Groups and Implicit Race Bias in the United States , 2015 .

[6]  Wei Wang,et al.  Multicultural experiences reduce intergroup bias through epistemic unfreezing. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[7]  Taezoon Park,et al.  When stereotypes meet robots: The double-edge sword of robot gender and personality in human-robot interaction , 2014, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[8]  Nalini Ambady,et al.  Continuous dynamics in the real-time perception of race , 2010 .

[9]  D. Gilbert,et al.  The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs. , 1991 .

[10]  Joshua Correll,et al.  The World Is Not Black and White: Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot in a Multiethnic Context , 2012 .

[11]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Consequences of Diversity for Social Cohesion and Prejudice: The Missing Dimension of Intergroup Contact , 2015 .

[12]  C. Bartneck,et al.  Comparing the Similarity of Responses Received from Studies in Amazon’s Mechanical Turk to Studies Conducted Online and with Direct Recruitment , 2015, PloS one.

[13]  Dana Kulic,et al.  Measurement Instruments for the Anthropomorphism, Animacy, Likeability, Perceived Intelligence, and Perceived Safety of Robots , 2009, Int. J. Soc. Robotics.

[14]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. , 1994 .

[15]  Christoph Bartneck,et al.  Anthropomorphism: Opportunities and Challenges in Human–Robot Interaction , 2014, International Journal of Social Robotics.

[16]  Christoph Bartneck,et al.  Robots And Racism , 2018, 2018 13th ACM/IEEE International Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI).

[17]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  of Experimental Psychology : General Reducing Implicit Racial Preferences : I . A Comparative Investigation of 17 Interventions , 2014 .

[18]  Virginia Contreras,et al.  Robots Racialized in the Likeness of Marginalized Social Identities are Subject to Greater Dehumanization than those racialized as White , 2018, 2018 27th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN).