Neurocognitive Modeling of Perceptual Decision Making
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] N. P. Bichot,et al. Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques , 1996, Nature.
[2] Michael L. Mack,et al. Modeling categorization of scenes containing consistent versus inconsistent objects. , 2010, Journal of vision.
[3] A. Tversky,et al. Choices, Values, and Frames , 2000 .
[4] R. Ratcliff,et al. Estimating parameters of the diffusion model: Approaches to dealing with contaminant reaction times and parameter variability , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[5] D. Meyer,et al. Neuroeconomics: The Consilience of Brain and Decision , 2004 .
[6] Eric Chown,et al. Cognitive Modeling , 2014, Computing Handbook, 3rd ed..
[7] Scott D. Brown,et al. The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.
[8] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Neural basis of the set-size effect in frontal eye field: timing of attention during visual search. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[9] Thomas J. Palmeri,et al. Memory systems and perceptual categorization , 2002 .
[10] Simon Farrell,et al. Computational Modeling in Cognition: Principles and Practice , 2010 .
[11] Jeremy M Wolfe,et al. What are the shapes of response time distributions in visual search? , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] J. Schall,et al. Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.
[13] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding: II. Feedback, expectation, olfaction, illusions , 1976, Biological Cybernetics.
[14] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. The neural systems that mediate human perceptual decision making , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[15] G. Woodman,et al. The Effect of Visual Search Efficiency on Response Preparation , 2008, Psychological science.
[16] P. Cisek,et al. Decisions in Changing Conditions: The Urgency-Gating Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[17] N. P. Bichot,et al. Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[18] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Neurally constrained modeling of perceptual decision making. , 2010, Psychological review.
[19] Scott D. Brown,et al. Neural Correlates of Trial-to-Trial Fluctuations in Response Caution , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] D. Vickers,et al. Evidence for an accumulator model of psychophysical discrimination. , 1970, Ergonomics.
[21] James L. McClelland,et al. The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.
[22] C. White,et al. Perceptual Criteria in the Human Brain , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] G. Logan,et al. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[24] Philip L. Smith. From Poisson shot noise to the integrated Ornstein–Uhlenbeck process: Neurally principled models of information accumulation in decision-making and response time , 2010 .
[25] G. Logan. Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .
[26] Birte U. Forstmann,et al. A Bayesian framework for simultaneously modeling neural and behavioral data , 2013, NeuroImage.
[27] W. Newsome,et al. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] J. Schall. On building a bridge between brain and behavior. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.
[29] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.
[30] M. Carandini. From circuits to behavior: a bridge too far? , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[31] Roger Ratcliff,et al. Parameter variability and distributional assumptions in the diffusion model. , 2013, Psychological review.
[32] C. Bruce,et al. Topography of projections to posterior cortical areas from the macaque frontal eye fields , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[33] Ehtibar N Dzhafarov,et al. Unfalsifiability and mutual translatability of major modeling schemes for choice reaction time. , 2014, Psychological review.
[34] R. Ratcliff,et al. A comparison of macaque behavior and superior colliculus neuronal activity to predictions from models of two-choice decisions. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] G. D. Logan,et al. Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque , 2007, Vision Research.
[36] P. Goldman-Rakic,et al. The primate mediodorsal (MD) nucleus and its projection to the frontal lobe , 1985, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[37] Chung-Chuan Lo,et al. Proactive Inhibitory Control and Attractor Dynamics in Countermanding Action: A Spiking Neural Circuit Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[38] Gordon D Logan,et al. Selecting a response in task switching: testing a model of compound cue retrieval. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[39] R. Nosofsky,et al. An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. , 1997, Psychological review.
[40] T. Palmeri. Exemplar similarity and the development of automaticity. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[41] C. Eriksen,et al. Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .
[42] T. Palmeri. An exemplar of model-based cognitive neuroscience , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[43] G. Logan,et al. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. , 2014, Psychological review.
[44] S. Liversedge,et al. Oxford handbook of eye movements , 2011 .
[45] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Neural chronometry and coherency across speed–accuracy demands reveal lack of homomorphism between computational and neural mechanisms of evidence accumulation , 2013, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[46] C. Bruce,et al. Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] R. Wurtz,et al. Interaction of the frontal eye field and superior colliculus for saccade generation. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[48] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .
[49] Patrick G. Bissett,et al. Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .
[50] Richard P. Heitz,et al. Response variability of frontal eye field neurons modulates with sensory input and saccade preparation but not visual search salience. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[51] Philip L. Smith,et al. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[52] Jochen Ditterich,et al. Stochastic models of decisions about motion direction: Behavior and physiology , 2006, Neural Networks.
[53] A. Pouget,et al. Variance as a Signature of Neural Computations during Decision Making , 2011, Neuron.
[54] David S. Brée,et al. Linking Propositions , 1986, COLING.
[55] Philip L. Smith,et al. Dual diffusion model for single-cell recording data from the superior colliculus in a brightness-discrimination task. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[56] G. Cottrell,et al. Modeling Perceptual Expertise , 2009 .
[57] C. Bruce,et al. Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.
[58] Ashby,et al. A Stochastic Version of General Recognition Theory. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[59] J T Townsend,et al. Truth and consequences of ordinal differences in statistical distributions: toward a theory of hierarchical inference. , 1990, Psychological bulletin.
[60] R. Nosofsky. Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[61] Aditya Murthy,et al. Neural control of visual search by frontal eye field: effects of unexpected target displacement on visual selection and saccade preparation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[62] Jochen Ditterich,et al. New advances in understanding decisions among multiple alternatives , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[63] Thomas J. Palmeri,et al. Visual Object Perception and Long-term Memory , 2008 .
[64] G. Logan. On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .
[65] W. Becker,et al. An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.
[66] KongFatt Wong-Lin,et al. Neural Circuit Dynamics Underlying Accumulation of Time-Varying Evidence During Perceptual Decision Making , 2007, Frontiers Comput. Neurosci..
[67] Braden A. Purcell,et al. From Salience to Saccades: Multiple-Alternative Gated Stochastic Accumulator Model of Visual Search , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[68] Takashi R Sato,et al. Effects of Stimulus-Response Compatibility on Neural Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2003, Neuron.
[69] J. Gold,et al. Banburismus and the Brain Decoding the Relationship between Sensory Stimuli, Decisions, and Reward , 2002, Neuron.
[70] Philip L. Smith,et al. A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time. , 2004, Psychological review.
[71] Jeremiah Y. Cohen,et al. The neural basis of saccade target selection , 1995 .
[72] J. Schall,et al. Visual and Motor Connectivity and the Distribution of Calcium-Binding Proteins in Macaque Frontal Eye Field: Implications for Saccade Target Selection , 2009, Front. Neuroanat..
[73] G. Logan,et al. Inhibitory control in mind and brain: an interactive race model of countermanding saccades. , 2007, Psychological review.
[74] I. Gauthier,et al. Computational approaches to the development of perceptual expertise , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[75] Jeffrey D. Schall,et al. CONCURRENT, DISTRIBUTED CONTROL OF SACCADE INITIATION IN THE FRONTAL EYE FIELD AND SUPERIOR COLLICULUS , 2003 .
[76] Gordon D Logan,et al. An instance theory of attention and memory. , 2002, Psychological review.
[77] R. D. Gordon,et al. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.
[78] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[79] Leanne Boucher,et al. Neural Basis of Adaptive Response Time Adjustment during Saccade Countermanding , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[80] D. Sparks. The brainstem control of saccadic eye movements , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[81] T. Zandt,et al. Time-dependent Poisson counter models of response latency in simple judgment. , 2000, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.
[82] J. Gold,et al. The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.
[83] M. Carandini,et al. Normalization as a canonical neural computation , 2013, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[84] R. Wurtz,et al. Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. IV. Relation of substantia nigra to superior colliculus. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.
[85] Scott D. Brown,et al. Domain General Mechanisms of Perceptual Decision Making in Human Cortex , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[86] Jochen Ditterich,et al. A Comparison between Mechanisms of Multi-Alternative Perceptual Decision Making: Ability to Explain Human Behavior, Predictions for Neurophysiology, and Relationship with Decision Theory , 2010, Front. Neurosci..
[87] Jeffrey D. Schall,et al. Neural basis of deciding, choosing and acting , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[88] Evan M. Palmer,et al. Reaction time distributions constrain models of visual search , 2010, Vision Research.
[89] Birte U. Forstmann,et al. Reciprocal relations between cognitive neuroscience and formal cognitive models: opposites attract? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[90] Philip L. Smith,et al. An integrated theory of attention and decision making in visual signal detection. , 2009, Psychological review.
[91] M. Shadlen,et al. A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[92] B. Love,et al. Learning the exception to the rule: model-based FMRI reveals specialized representations for surprising category members. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.
[93] Leanne Boucher,et al. Nonindependent and nonstationary response times in stopping and stepping saccade tasks , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[94] R. Ratcliff,et al. Neural Representation of Task Difficulty and Decision Making during Perceptual Categorization: A Timing Diagram , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[95] Konstantinos V. Katsikopoulos. "How to model it?" [Review of the book Cognitive modeling by J. R. Busemeyer & A. Diederich, Los Angeles: Sage, 2010] , 2011 .
[96] T. Zandt,et al. How to fit a response time distribution , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[97] Leanne Boucher,et al. Stopping eye and hand movements: Are the processes independent? , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[98] J. Townsend,et al. Decision field theory: a dynamic-cognitive approach to decision making in an uncertain environment. , 1993, Psychological review.
[99] G. Logan,et al. Inhibitory control in mind and brain 2.0: blocked-input models of saccadic countermanding. , 2015, Psychological review.
[100] Xiao-Jing Wang,et al. Cortico–basal ganglia circuit mechanism for a decision threshold in reaction time tasks , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[101] D. Teller. Linking propositions , 1984, Vision Research.
[102] Takashi R Sato,et al. Search Efficiency but Not Response Interference Affects Visual Selection in Frontal Eye Field , 2001, Neuron.
[103] Darryl W. Schneider,et al. Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[104] Braden A Purcell,et al. Response times from ensembles of accumulators , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[105] GrossbergS.. Adaptive pattern classification and universal recoding , 1976 .
[106] J. Schall,et al. Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[107] Donald Laming,et al. Information theory of choice-reaction times , 1968 .
[108] E. Wagenmakers,et al. An Introduction to Model-Based Cognitive Neuroscience , 2015, Springer New York.
[109] J. Bullier,et al. Topography of visual cortex connections with frontal eye field in macaque: convergence and segregation of processing streams , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[110] Takashi R Sato,et al. Neuronal Basis of Covert Spatial Attention in the Frontal Eye Field , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[111] D. P. Hanes,et al. Controlled Movement Processing: Superior Colliculus Activity Associated with Countermanded Saccades , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[112] David Ferrier,et al. The localization of function in the brain , 1874, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.
[113] James T. Townsend. Truth and consequences of ordinal differences in statistical distributions: toward a theory of hierarchical inference. , 1990 .
[114] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.
[115] A. Fuchs,et al. The brainstem burst generator for saccadic eye movements , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.
[116] P. Glimcher,et al. Neuroeconomics: The Consilience of Brain and Decision , 2004, Science.
[117] N. P. Bichot,et al. Reliability of Macaque Frontal Eye Field Neurons Signaling Saccade Targets during Visual Search , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[118] Roger Ratcliff,et al. A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .
[119] P. Dayan,et al. Decision theory, reinforcement learning, and the brain , 2008, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[120] N. P. Bichot,et al. Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[121] Michael L. Mack,et al. The Timing of Visual Object Categorization , 2011, Front. Psychology.