Postconditioning devaluation of a reinforcer affects instrumental responding.

[1]  E. Tolman Sign-Gestalt or conditioned reflex. , 1933 .

[2]  N. Miller A reply to 'Sign-Gestalt or Conditioned Reflex?' , 1935 .

[3]  B. Skinner,et al.  Principles of Behavior , 1944 .

[4]  H. Gleitman,et al.  Studies in learning and motivation; equal reinforcements in both end-boxes; followed by shock in one end-box. , 1949, Journal of experimental psychology.

[5]  J. H. Curtis,et al.  Learning Theory and Behavior , 1960 .

[6]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[7]  E. E. Krieckhaus,et al.  Acquisition of sodium by rats: interaction of innate mechanisms and latent learning. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[8]  The effect of satiation following partial reinforcement , 1970 .

[9]  R. Kovner,et al.  Cue properties vs palatability of flavors in avoidance learning , 1970 .

[10]  Some parameters of latent learning and generalized drives. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[11]  F. W. Irwin Intentional behavior and motivation : a cognitive theory , 1971 .

[12]  R. Bolles Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. , 1972 .

[13]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[14]  J. Falk Physiology and Behavior. , 1973 .

[15]  G. Morrison,et al.  Taste-mediated conditioned aversion to an exteroceptive stimulus following LiCl poisoning. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[16]  M. Morgan Resistance to satiation , 1974 .

[17]  E. Holman Some conditions for the dissociation of consummatory and instrumental behavior in rats , 1975 .

[18]  R. Rescorla,et al.  The effect of two ways of devaluing the unconditioned stimulus after first- and second-order appetitive conditioning. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[19]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Handbook of Operant Behavior , 2022 .

[20]  E. Capaldi,et al.  Resistance to satiation of consummatory and instrumental performance. , 1978 .

[21]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Within-compound flavor associations. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[22]  L. Nilsson,et al.  Role of exteroceptive background context in taste-aversion conditioning and extinction , 1979 .

[23]  W. K. Honig,et al.  Cognitive Processes in Animal Behavior , 1979 .

[24]  A. Amsel,et al.  Recall (versus recognition) of taste and immunization against aversive taste anticipations based on illness. , 1980, Science.

[25]  Christopher D. Adams Post-Conditioning Devaluation of an Instrumental Reinforcer has no Effect on Extinction Performance , 1980 .

[26]  Christopher D. Adams,et al.  Instrumental Responding following Reinforcer Devaluation , 1981 .

[27]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Sensory specific satiety in man , 1981, Physiology & Behavior.

[28]  J. E. Sherman,et al.  Aversion to the reinforcer differentially affects conditioned reinforcement and instrumental responding. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[29]  Christopher D. Adams Variations in the Sensitivity of Instrumental Responding to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1982 .

[30]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Irrelevant Incentive Learning during Instrumental Conditioning: The Role of the Drive-Reinforcer and Response-Reinforcer Relationships , 1983 .

[31]  Christopher D. Adams,et al.  The Effect of the Instrumental Training Contingency on Susceptibility to Reinforcer Devaluation , 1983 .

[32]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Irrelevant Incentive Learning during Training on Ratio and Interval Schedules , 1983 .

[33]  M. Spetch,et al.  Variety in the flavor of food enhances eating in the rat: A controlled demonstration , 1983, Physiology & Behavior.

[34]  R. Claire-Smith,et al.  Response preconditioning effects. , 1983 .

[35]  D. G. Mook,et al.  Glucose solution intake in the rat: The specificity of postingestive satiety , 1983, Appetite.