What inhibits the mere-exposure effect: Recollection or familiarity?
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Schacter. Implicit memory: History and current status. , 1987 .
[2] D. Schacter,et al. Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[3] Preference and expectancy arousal: further evidence. , 1970, The Journal of general psychology.
[4] N. Brody,et al. Critical importance of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[5] P. Barchas,et al. Processing of preconsciously acquired information measured by hemispheric asymmetry and selection accuracy. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.
[6] John G. Seamon,et al. The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: Effects of stimulus type, encoding conditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affect judgments. , 1995 .
[7] Franziska Marquart,et al. Communication and persuasion : central and peripheral routes to attitude change , 1988 .
[8] Marc Vanhuele,et al. Mere Exposure and the Cognitive-Affective Debate Revisited , 1994 .
[9] J. M. Nuttin. Affective consequences of mere ownership: The name letter effect in twelve European languages , 1987 .
[10] D. Berlyne,et al. Aesthetics and Psychobiology , 1975 .
[11] A. Yonelinas. Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: evidence for a dual-process model. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[12] P. Pliner. The Effects of Mere Exposure on Liking for Edible Substances , 1982, Appetite.
[13] D. Berlyne. Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value , 1970 .
[14] R. Moreland,et al. Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects , 1977 .
[15] Serge Moscovici,et al. Current issues in European social psychology , 1983 .
[16] R. Porter,et al. Olfactory preferences resulting from mere exposure in human neonates , 1986 .
[17] W. Wilson,et al. Feeling more than we can know: Exposure effects without learning. , 1979 .
[18] George Mandler,et al. Nonspecific Effects of Exposure on Stimuli That Cannot Be Recognized , 1987 .
[19] J. Kihlstrom. The cognitive unconscious. , 1987, Science.
[20] Richard E. Petty,et al. Repetition, Cognitive Responses and Persuasion , 1981 .
[21] W. F. V. Raaij. Affective and cognitive reactions to advertising , 1984 .
[22] R. Bornstein. Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. , 1989 .
[23] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[24] Carl Obermiller,et al. Varieties of Mere Exposure: The Effects of Processing Style and Repetition on Affective Response , 1985 .
[25] G. Bonanno,et al. Preference, familiarity, and recognition after repeated brief exposures to random geometric shapes. , 1986, The American journal of psychology.
[26] Andrew P. Yonelinas,et al. Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: measuring recollection , 1993 .
[27] G. Mandler,et al. Aspects of Consciousness , 1987 .
[28] Chris Janiszewski. Preattentive Mere Exposure Effects , 1993 .
[29] L. Cooper,et al. Implicit memory for possible and impossible objects: constraints on the construction of structural descriptions. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[30] W. Fred van Raaij,et al. How Consumers React to Advertising , 1989 .
[31] Stewart Shapiro,et al. Comparing implicit and explicit memory for brand names from advertisements. , 1996 .
[32] P. Graf,et al. Process dissociation procedure: Core assumptions fail, sometimes , 1995 .
[33] R. Zajonc. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. , 1968 .
[34] S. Lewandowsky,et al. Implicit Memory: Theoretical Issues , 1989 .
[35] D. Schacter,et al. Intention, awareness, and implicit memory: The retrieval intentionality criterion , 1989 .
[36] M W Matlin,et al. Response competition, recognition, and affect. , 1971, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[37] Larry L. Jacoby,et al. In defense of functional independence: Violations of assumptions underlying the process-dissociation procedure? , 1997 .
[38] L. Jacoby. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .
[39] R. Zajonc,et al. Additivity of nonconscious affect: combined effects of priming and exposure. , 1995, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[40] D. Stang. Intuition as artifact in mere exposure studies. , 1974 .
[41] E M Reingold,et al. Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[42] D. Newlin,et al. Nonassociative mechanisms in preferences for alcoholic flavors: differences between sons of alcoholics and sons of nonalcoholics. , 1991, Addictive behaviors.
[43] J. E. Grush. A Summary Review of Mediating Explanations of Exposure Phenomena , 1979 .
[44] A. Greenwald,et al. Audience Involvement in Advertising: Four Levels , 1984 .
[45] John G. Seamon,et al. Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: effects of shadowing, masking, and cerebral laterality , 1983 .
[46] R. Zajonc,et al. Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. , 1980, Science.