Inhibition in selective attention
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Ali Jannati,et al. Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] Mark G. Stokes,et al. Selective inhibition of distracting input , 2017, Behavioural Brain Research.
[3] John M. Gaspar,et al. Suppression of Salient Objects Prevents Distraction in Visual Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] G. DeAngelis,et al. Neural Correlates of Prior Expectations of Motion in the Lateral Intraparietal and Middle Temporal Areas , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] Bradley R. Postle,et al. Decoding and Reconstructing the Focus of Spatial Attention from the Topography of Alpha-band Oscillations , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] J. D. McGaugh. Memory--a century of consolidation. , 2000, Science.
[7] G. Thut,et al. Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α‐band EEG synchronization , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.
[8] M. Turatto,et al. Filtering visual onsets via habituation: A context-specific long-term memory of irrelevant stimuli , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[9] Hermann J. Müller,et al. Attentional capture in visual search: Capture and post-capture dynamics revealed by EEG , 2017, NeuroImage.
[10] Jan Theeuwes,et al. In competition for the attentional template: can multiple items within visual working memory guide attention? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[11] P. Roelfsema,et al. Different States in Visual Working Memory: When It Guides Attention and When It Does Not , 2022 .
[12] L. Saksida,et al. Visual perception and memory: a new view of medial temporal lobe function in primates and rodents. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.
[13] G. V. Simpson,et al. Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[14] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Bayesian inference and attentional modulation in the visual cortex , 2005, Neuroreport.
[15] T. Moore,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. , 2017, Annual review of psychology.
[16] Robert M. Mok,et al. Causal implication by rhythmic transcranial magnetic stimulation of alpha frequency in feature‐based local vs. global attention , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[17] L. Chelazzi,et al. Getting rid of visual distractors: the why, when, how, and where. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[18] Carol A. Seger,et al. The Basal Ganglia in Human Learning , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[19] Nancy B. Carlisle,et al. Location-based explanations do not account for active attentional suppression , 2019, Visual Cognition.
[20] W. Usrey,et al. Preparatory Effects of Distractor Suppression: Evidence from Visual Cortex , 2011, PloS one.
[21] A. Nobre,et al. Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[22] S. Kastner,et al. From Behavior to Neural Dynamics: An Integrated Theory of Attention , 2015, Neuron.
[23] Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al. LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[24] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2013, J. Cogn. Neurosci..
[25] S. Luck,et al. Dissociable Decoding of Spatial Attention and Working Memory from EEG Oscillations and Sustained Potentials , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[26] Hsuan-Fu Chao. Top-down attentional control for distractor locations: the benefit of precuing distractor locations on target localization and discrimination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Steven J Luck,et al. Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[28] G. Mangun,et al. Signal enhancement and suppression during visual–spatial selective attention , 2010, Brain Research.
[29] E. N. Sokolov. Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. , 1963, Annual review of physiology.
[30] Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al. Cortical Mechanisms of Prioritizing Selection for Rejection in Visual Search , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[31] Nicholas B. Turk-Browne,et al. Associative Prediction of Visual Shape in the Hippocampus , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[32] Stefan Pollmann,et al. No evidence for enhanced distractor template representation in early visual cortex , 2018, Cortex.
[33] Steven J. Luck,et al. The Role of Inhibition in Avoiding Distraction by Salient Stimuli , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] John J. Foxe,et al. The functional role of alpha-band activity in attentional processing: the current zeitgeist and future outlook. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[35] Nicholas Gaspelin,et al. Inhibition as a potential resolution to the attentional capture debate. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[36] David Pascucci,et al. Short-term and long-term plasticity in the visual-attention system: Evidence from habituation of attentional capture , 2016, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[37] G. Piantoni,et al. Alpha Power Predicts Persistence of Bistable Perception , 2017, Scientific Reports.
[38] L. Itti,et al. Search Goal Tunes Visual Features Optimally , 2007, Neuron.
[39] Heinrich René Liesefeld,et al. Probability cueing of singleton-distractor locations in visual search: priority-map‐ or dimension-based inhibition? , 2018, bioRxiv.
[40] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[41] H. Egeth,et al. Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[42] J. O'Keefe,et al. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.
[43] N. P. Bichot,et al. A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.
[44] O. Jensen,et al. Rhythmic Pulsing: Linking Ongoing Brain Activity with Evoked Responses , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[45] Jon Driver,et al. Attentional Preparation for a Lateralized Visual Distractor: Behavioral and fMRI Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[46] John J. Foxe,et al. Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[47] Dominique Lamy,et al. Effects of task relevance and stimulus-driven salience in feature-search mode. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[48] R. Cohen Kadosh,et al. Electrical Stimulation of Alpha Oscillations Stabilizes Performance on Visual Attention Tasks , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[49] Marius V Peelen,et al. Preparatory attention in visual cortex , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[50] Matthew D. Weaver,et al. How do we ignore salient distractors , 2017 .
[51] H J Müller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.
[52] Steven J. Luck,et al. Electrophysiological Correlates of the Focusing of Attention within Complex Visual Scenes: N2pc and Related ERP Components , 2011 .
[53] Joy J Geng,et al. Distractor Probability Changes the Shape of the Attentional Template , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[54] Steven J Luck,et al. Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[55] Brett Bahle,et al. Feature-Based Statistical Regularities of Distractors Modulate Attentional Capture , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Statistical regularities induce spatial as well as feature-specific suppression. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] Benjamin J. Kraus,et al. Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration , 2013, Neuron.
[58] Jeffrey D. Schall,et al. Prefrontal Control of Visual Distraction , 2017, Current Biology.
[59] M. Stokes. ‘Activity-silent’ working memory in prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[60] Richard F. Thompson. Habituation: A history , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[61] David Pascucci,et al. The distracting impact of repeated visible and invisible onsets on focused attention. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] Donald A. Wilson,et al. Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[63] Howard E Egeth,et al. The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[64] H. Eichenbaum. A cortical–hippocampal system for declarative memory , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[65] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Anticipatory distractor suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search , 2019, bioRxiv.
[66] Lynne M Reder,et al. The adaptive character of the attentional system: statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[67] W. Klimesch. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[68] D. Heeger,et al. The Normalization Model of Attention , 2009, Neuron.
[69] Carl C. H. Petersen,et al. Higher Nervous Functions , 2007 .
[70] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Learning to suppress a distractor is not affected by working memory load , 2019, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.
[71] H. J. Muller,et al. Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[72] J. Theeuwes. Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[73] Steven J. Luck,et al. Combined Electrophysiological and Behavioral Evidence for the Suppression of Salient Distractors , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[74] Christopher Summerfield,et al. Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2016, Current Biology.
[75] G. Mangun,et al. The role of the pulvinar in distractor processing and visual search , 2013, Human brain mapping.
[76] S. Luck,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.
[77] Michael M. Halassa,et al. Prefrontal Cortex Regulates Sensory Filtering through a Basal Ganglia-to-Thalamus Pathway , 2019, Neuron.
[78] Mark W. Becker,et al. No templates for rejection: a failure to configure attention to ignore task-irrelevant features , 2015 .
[79] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[80] Heleen A. Slagter,et al. Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[81] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[82] O. Jensen,et al. Modulation of Gamma and Alpha Activity during a Working Memory Task Engaging the Dorsal or Ventral Stream , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[83] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. The role of relational information in contingent capture. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[84] Cooper A. Smout,et al. Attention promotes the neural encoding of prediction errors , 2019, bioRxiv.
[85] Theodore P. Zanto,et al. Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[86] Mark F. Bear,et al. Visual recognition memory, manifest as long-term habituation, requires synaptic plasticity in V1 , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.
[87] Joy J. Geng,et al. Attentional Mechanisms of Distractor Suppression , 2014 .
[88] G. Woodman,et al. Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[89] Michael X. Cohen,et al. Attention and Temporal Expectations Modulate Power, Not Phase, of Ongoing Alpha Oscillations , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[90] R. VanRullen,et al. The Phase of Ongoing EEG Oscillations Predicts Visual Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[91] Stefan Pollmann,et al. Cortical evidence for negative search templates , 2017 .
[92] Howard E Egeth,et al. Taming the White Bear , 2016, Psychological science.
[93] John J. Foxe,et al. Anticipatory Attentional Suppression of Visual Features Indexed by Oscillatory Alpha-Band Power Increases:A High-Density Electrical Mapping Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[94] E. John,et al. Perceptual framing and cortical alpha rhythm , 1981, Neuropsychologia.
[95] O. Jensen,et al. Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against Anticipated Distracters , 2012, Current Biology.
[96] Christian N. L. Olivers,et al. Oscillatory Mechanisms of Preparing for Visual Distraction , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[97] M. Behrmann,et al. Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[98] Edward Awh,et al. Alpha-Band Oscillations Enable Spatially and Temporally Resolved Tracking of Covert Spatial Attention , 2017, Psychological science.
[99] David W. Sutterer,et al. The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.
[100] J. Gottlieb,et al. Distinct neural mechanisms of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal lobe , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[101] Paul M Corballis,et al. Event-related potentials reveal the effect of prior knowledge on competition for representation and attentional capture. , 2014, Psychophysiology.
[102] M. Chun,et al. Concurrent working memory load can facilitate selective attention: evidence for specialized load. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[103] C. Koch,et al. Is perception discrete or continuous? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[104] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[105] M. Turatto,et al. Habituation of Oculomotor Capture by Sudden Onsets: Stimulus Specificity, Spontaneous Recovery and Dishabituation , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[106] Edward Awh,et al. Spatially Selective Alpha Oscillations Reveal Moment-by-Moment Trade-offs between Working Memory and Attention , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[107] L. Chelazzi,et al. Desensitizing the Attention System to Distraction While Idling: A New Latent Learning Phenomenon in the Visual Attention Domain , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[108] J. Geng,et al. The Attentional Template Is Shifted and Asymmetrically Sharpened by Distractor Context , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[109] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[110] R. Romo,et al. α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[111] Hidehiko Komatsu,et al. Target Selection in Area V4 during a Multidimensional Visual Search Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[112] Hermann J Müller,et al. Region-based shielding of visual search from salient distractors: Target detection is impaired with same- but not different-dimension distractors , 2018, Attention, perception & psychophysics.
[113] Leonardo Chelazzi,et al. Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering , 2017, Cortex.
[114] Andrew B. Leber,et al. It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[115] Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al. Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. , 1999 .
[116] B. Postle,et al. Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[117] Hermann J Müller,et al. Probability cueing of singleton-distractor locations in visual search: Priority-map- versus dimension-based inhibition? , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[118] Ben M. Crittenden,et al. Distinct Mechanisms for Distractor Suppression and Target Facilitation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[119] Anna Schubö,et al. Cueing distraction: electrophysiological evidence for anticipatory active suppression of distractor location , 2019, Psychological Research.
[120] J. Theeuwes. Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[121] Simon van Gaal,et al. No Evidence that Predictions and Attention Modulate the First Feedforward Sweep of Cortical Information Processing , 2018, bioRxiv.
[122] Vincent Di Lollo,et al. Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[123] Nuttida Rungratsameetaweemana,et al. Expectations Do Not Alter Early Sensory Processing during Perceptual Decision-Making , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[124] Jan Theeuwes,et al. How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[125] James W Bisley,et al. The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[126] Anders Petersen,et al. Attentional Capture by Salient Distractors during Visual Search Is Determined by Temporal Task Demands , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[127] Andrew Hollingworth,et al. Evidence for negative feature guidance in visual search is explained by spatial recoding. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[128] Karl J. Friston. The free-energy principle: a rough guide to the brain? , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[129] H. Müller,et al. Distractor handling via dimension weighting. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[130] Rufin VanRullen,et al. On the cyclic nature of perception in vision versus audition , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[131] Hassana K. Oyibo,et al. Experience-dependent spatial expectations in mouse visual cortex , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.
[132] Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al. Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.
[133] Edward Awh,et al. The role of alpha oscillations in spatial attention: limited evidence for a suppression account. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[134] L. Squire,et al. The medial temporal lobe memory system , 1991, Science.
[135] J. Serences,et al. Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orienting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[136] H. Müller,et al. Feature guidance by negative attentional templates depends on search difficulty , 2019, Visual Cognition.
[137] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Spatial suppression due to statistical regularities is driven by distractor suppression not by target activation , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[138] Shih-Cheng Yen,et al. Mixed selectivity morphs population codes in prefrontal cortex , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.
[139] Leonardo Chelazzi,et al. Orchestrating Proactive and Reactive Mechanisms for Filtering Distracting Information: Brain-Behavior Relationships Revealed by a Mixed-Design fMRI Study , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[140] Karl J. Friston,et al. Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[141] Joy J Geng,et al. Template-to-distractor distinctiveness regulates visual search efficiency. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.
[142] Dirk Kerzel,et al. Attentional capture during visual search is attenuated by target predictability: evidence from the N2pc, Pd, and topographic segmentation. , 2013, Psychophysiology.
[143] Clayton Hickey,et al. A temporal dependency account of attentional inhibition in oculomotor control , 2017, NeuroImage.
[144] J. Theeuwes,et al. Cueing the location of a distractor: an inhibitory mechanism of spatial attention? , 2008, Acta psychologica.
[145] H. Egeth,et al. The power of negative thinking: Paradoxical but effective ignoring of salient-but-irrelevant stimuli with a spatial cue , 2018, Visual Cognition.
[146] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Statistical Regularities Modulate Attentional Capture , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[147] S. Yantis,et al. Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[148] Daniel B. Vatterott,et al. Experience-dependent attentional tuning of distractor rejection , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[149] O. Jensen,et al. Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[150] Jason T. Arita,et al. Templates for rejection: configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[151] Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al. The lateral intraparietal area as a salience map: the representation of abrupt onset, stimulus motion, and task relevance , 2000, Vision Research.
[152] Joy J. Geng,et al. Evidence for Second-Order Singleton Suppression Based on Probabilistic Expectations , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[153] Floris P de Lange,et al. Prior expectations induce prestimulus sensory templates , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[154] M. Eimer. The neural basis of attentional control in visual search , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[155] John J. Foxe,et al. The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[156] Yuhong V Jiang,et al. Rapid acquisition but slow extinction of an attentional bias in space. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[157] Karl J. Friston,et al. A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[158] Nancy B. Carlisle,et al. Distractor Ignoring: Strategies, Learning, and Passive Filtering , 2019, Current directions in psychological science.
[159] Diane M. Beck,et al. To See or Not to See: Prestimulus α Phase Predicts Visual Awareness , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[160] S. Luck,et al. A Common Neural Mechanism for Preventing and Terminating the Allocation of Attention , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[161] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.