Inhibition in selective attention

Our ability to focus on goal‐relevant aspects of the environment is critically dependent on our ability to ignore or inhibit distracting information. One perspective is that distractor inhibition is under similar voluntary control as attentional facilitation of target processing. However, a rapidly growing body of research shows that distractor inhibition often relies on prior experience with the distracting information or other mechanisms that need not rely on active representation in working memory. Yet, how and when these different forms of inhibition are neurally implemented remains largely unclear. Here, we review findings from recent behavioral and neuroimaging studies to address this outstanding question. We specifically explore how experience with distracting information may change the processing of that information in the context of current predictive processing views of perception: by modulating a distractor's representation already in anticipation of the distractor, or after integration of top‐down and bottom‐up sensory signals. We also outline directions for future research necessary to enhance our understanding of how the brain filters out distracting information.

[1]  Ali Jannati,et al.  Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Mark G. Stokes,et al.  Selective inhibition of distracting input , 2017, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  John M. Gaspar,et al.  Suppression of Salient Objects Prevents Distraction in Visual Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Neural Correlates of Prior Expectations of Motion in the Lateral Intraparietal and Middle Temporal Areas , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Decoding and Reconstructing the Focus of Spatial Attention from the Topography of Alpha-band Oscillations , 2016, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  J. D. McGaugh Memory--a century of consolidation. , 2000, Science.

[7]  G. Thut,et al.  Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α‐band EEG synchronization , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[8]  M. Turatto,et al.  Filtering visual onsets via habituation: A context-specific long-term memory of irrelevant stimuli , 2017, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[9]  Hermann J. Müller,et al.  Attentional capture in visual search: Capture and post-capture dynamics revealed by EEG , 2017, NeuroImage.

[10]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  In competition for the attentional template: can multiple items within visual working memory guide attention? , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  P. Roelfsema,et al.  Different States in Visual Working Memory: When It Guides Attention and When It Does Not , 2022 .

[12]  L. Saksida,et al.  Visual perception and memory: a new view of medial temporal lobe function in primates and rodents. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[13]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Bayesian inference and attentional modulation in the visual cortex , 2005, Neuroreport.

[15]  T. Moore,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. , 2017, Annual review of psychology.

[16]  Robert M. Mok,et al.  Causal implication by rhythmic transcranial magnetic stimulation of alpha frequency in feature‐based local vs. global attention , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[17]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Getting rid of visual distractors: the why, when, how, and where. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[18]  Carol A. Seger,et al.  The Basal Ganglia in Human Learning , 2006, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[19]  Nancy B. Carlisle,et al.  Location-based explanations do not account for active attentional suppression , 2019, Visual Cognition.

[20]  W. Usrey,et al.  Preparatory Effects of Distractor Suppression: Evidence from Visual Cortex , 2011, PloS one.

[21]  A. Nobre,et al.  Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[22]  S. Kastner,et al.  From Behavior to Neural Dynamics: An Integrated Theory of Attention , 2015, Neuron.

[23]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence of the Capture of Visual Attention , 2013, J. Cogn. Neurosci..

[25]  S. Luck,et al.  Dissociable Decoding of Spatial Attention and Working Memory from EEG Oscillations and Sustained Potentials , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Hsuan-Fu Chao Top-down attentional control for distractor locations: the benefit of precuing distractor locations on target localization and discrimination. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[28]  G. Mangun,et al.  Signal enhancement and suppression during visual–spatial selective attention , 2010, Brain Research.

[29]  E. N. Sokolov Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. , 1963, Annual review of physiology.

[30]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Cortical Mechanisms of Prioritizing Selection for Rejection in Visual Search , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Nicholas B. Turk-Browne,et al.  Associative Prediction of Visual Shape in the Hippocampus , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  No evidence for enhanced distractor template representation in early visual cortex , 2018, Cortex.

[33]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  The Role of Inhibition in Avoiding Distraction by Salient Stimuli , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[34]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The functional role of alpha-band activity in attentional processing: the current zeitgeist and future outlook. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[35]  Nicholas Gaspelin,et al.  Inhibition as a potential resolution to the attentional capture debate. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[36]  David Pascucci,et al.  Short-term and long-term plasticity in the visual-attention system: Evidence from habituation of attentional capture , 2016, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[37]  G. Piantoni,et al.  Alpha Power Predicts Persistence of Bistable Perception , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[38]  L. Itti,et al.  Search Goal Tunes Visual Features Optimally , 2007, Neuron.

[39]  Heinrich René Liesefeld,et al.  Probability cueing of singleton-distractor locations in visual search: priority-map‐ or dimension-based inhibition? , 2018, bioRxiv.

[40]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[41]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[42]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[43]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[44]  O. Jensen,et al.  Rhythmic Pulsing: Linking Ongoing Brain Activity with Evoked Responses , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[45]  Jon Driver,et al.  Attentional Preparation for a Lateralized Visual Distractor: Behavioral and fMRI Evidence , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Dominique Lamy,et al.  Effects of task relevance and stimulus-driven salience in feature-search mode. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  R. Cohen Kadosh,et al.  Electrical Stimulation of Alpha Oscillations Stabilizes Performance on Visual Attention Tasks , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  Marius V Peelen,et al.  Preparatory attention in visual cortex , 2017, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[50]  Matthew D. Weaver,et al.  How do we ignore salient distractors , 2017 .

[51]  H J Müller,et al.  Visual search for singleton feature targets within and across feature dimensions , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[52]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological Correlates of the Focusing of Attention within Complex Visual Scenes: N2pc and Related ERP Components , 2011 .

[53]  Joy J Geng,et al.  Distractor Probability Changes the Shape of the Attentional Template , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention , 2012, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[55]  Brett Bahle,et al.  Feature-Based Statistical Regularities of Distractors Modulate Attentional Capture , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Statistical regularities induce spatial as well as feature-specific suppression. , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[57]  Benjamin J. Kraus,et al.  Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration , 2013, Neuron.

[58]  Jeffrey D. Schall,et al.  Prefrontal Control of Visual Distraction , 2017, Current Biology.

[59]  M. Stokes ‘Activity-silent’ working memory in prefrontal cortex: a dynamic coding framework , 2015, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Richard F. Thompson Habituation: A history , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[61]  David Pascucci,et al.  The distracting impact of repeated visible and invisible onsets on focused attention. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  Donald A. Wilson,et al.  Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation , 2009, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[63]  Howard E Egeth,et al.  The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[64]  H. Eichenbaum A cortical–hippocampal system for declarative memory , 2000, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[65]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Anticipatory distractor suppression elicited by statistical regularities in visual search , 2019, bioRxiv.

[66]  Lynne M Reder,et al.  The adaptive character of the attentional system: statistical sensitivity in a target localization task. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[67]  W. Klimesch Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[68]  D. Heeger,et al.  The Normalization Model of Attention , 2009, Neuron.

[69]  Carl C. H. Petersen,et al.  Higher Nervous Functions , 2007 .

[70]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Learning to suppress a distractor is not affected by working memory load , 2019, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[71]  H. J. Muller,et al.  Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[72]  J. Theeuwes Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[73]  Steven J. Luck,et al.  Combined Electrophysiological and Behavioral Evidence for the Suppression of Salient Distractors , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[74]  Christopher Summerfield,et al.  Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2016, Current Biology.

[75]  G. Mangun,et al.  The role of the pulvinar in distractor processing and visual search , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[76]  S. Luck,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. , 1994, Psychophysiology.

[77]  Michael M. Halassa,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Regulates Sensory Filtering through a Basal Ganglia-to-Thalamus Pathway , 2019, Neuron.

[78]  Mark W. Becker,et al.  No templates for rejection: a failure to configure attention to ignore task-irrelevant features , 2015 .

[79]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  The neural basis of biased competition in human visual cortex , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[80]  Heleen A. Slagter,et al.  Learning What Is Irrelevant or Relevant: Expectations Facilitate Distractor Inhibition and Target Facilitation through Distinct Neural Mechanisms , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[81]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[82]  O. Jensen,et al.  Modulation of Gamma and Alpha Activity during a Working Memory Task Engaging the Dorsal or Ventral Stream , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[83]  Stefanie I. Becker,et al.  The role of relational information in contingent capture. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[84]  Cooper A. Smout,et al.  Attention promotes the neural encoding of prediction errors , 2019, bioRxiv.

[85]  Theodore P. Zanto,et al.  Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[86]  Mark F. Bear,et al.  Visual recognition memory, manifest as long-term habituation, requires synaptic plasticity in V1 , 2015, Nature Neuroscience.

[87]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Attentional Mechanisms of Distractor Suppression , 2014 .

[88]  G. Woodman,et al.  Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional selection during visual search? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[89]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Attention and Temporal Expectations Modulate Power, Not Phase, of Ongoing Alpha Oscillations , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[90]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The Phase of Ongoing EEG Oscillations Predicts Visual Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[91]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  Cortical evidence for negative search templates , 2017 .

[92]  Howard E Egeth,et al.  Taming the White Bear , 2016, Psychological science.

[93]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Anticipatory Attentional Suppression of Visual Features Indexed by Oscillatory Alpha-Band Power Increases:A High-Density Electrical Mapping Study , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[94]  E. John,et al.  Perceptual framing and cortical alpha rhythm , 1981, Neuropsychologia.

[95]  O. Jensen,et al.  Alpha Oscillations Serve to Protect Working Memory Maintenance against Anticipated Distracters , 2012, Current Biology.

[96]  Christian N. L. Olivers,et al.  Oscillatory Mechanisms of Preparing for Visual Distraction , 2019, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[97]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Spatial probability as an attentional cue in visual search , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[98]  Edward Awh,et al.  Alpha-Band Oscillations Enable Spatially and Temporally Resolved Tracking of Covert Spatial Attention , 2017, Psychological science.

[99]  David W. Sutterer,et al.  The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory. , 2016, Journal of neurophysiology.

[100]  J. Gottlieb,et al.  Distinct neural mechanisms of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal lobe , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[101]  Paul M Corballis,et al.  Event-related potentials reveal the effect of prior knowledge on competition for representation and attentional capture. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[102]  M. Chun,et al.  Concurrent working memory load can facilitate selective attention: evidence for specialized load. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[103]  C. Koch,et al.  Is perception discrete or continuous? , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[104]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[105]  M. Turatto,et al.  Habituation of Oculomotor Capture by Sudden Onsets: Stimulus Specificity, Spontaneous Recovery and Dishabituation , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[106]  Edward Awh,et al.  Spatially Selective Alpha Oscillations Reveal Moment-by-Moment Trade-offs between Working Memory and Attention , 2018, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[107]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Desensitizing the Attention System to Distraction While Idling: A New Latent Learning Phenomenon in the Visual Attention Domain , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[108]  J. Geng,et al.  The Attentional Template Is Shifted and Asymmetrically Sharpened by Distractor Context , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[109]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[110]  R. Romo,et al.  α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[111]  Hidehiko Komatsu,et al.  Target Selection in Area V4 during a Multidimensional Visual Search Task , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[112]  Hermann J Müller,et al.  Region-based shielding of visual search from salient distractors: Target detection is impaired with same- but not different-dimension distractors , 2018, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[113]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering , 2017, Cortex.

[114]  Andrew B. Leber,et al.  It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[115]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Predictive coding in the visual cortex: a functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. , 1999 .

[116]  B. Postle,et al.  Top-down control of the phase of alpha-band oscillations as a mechanism for temporal prediction , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[117]  Hermann J Müller,et al.  Probability cueing of singleton-distractor locations in visual search: Priority-map- versus dimension-based inhibition? , 2019, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[118]  Ben M. Crittenden,et al.  Distinct Mechanisms for Distractor Suppression and Target Facilitation , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[119]  Anna Schubö,et al.  Cueing distraction: electrophysiological evidence for anticipatory active suppression of distractor location , 2019, Psychological Research.

[120]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[121]  Simon van Gaal,et al.  No Evidence that Predictions and Attention Modulate the First Feedforward Sweep of Cortical Information Processing , 2018, bioRxiv.

[122]  Vincent Di Lollo,et al.  Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[123]  Nuttida Rungratsameetaweemana,et al.  Expectations Do Not Alter Early Sensory Processing during Perceptual Decision-Making , 2018, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[124]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  How to inhibit a distractor location? Statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[125]  James W Bisley,et al.  The what, where, and why of priority maps and their interactions with visual working memory , 2015, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[126]  Anders Petersen,et al.  Attentional Capture by Salient Distractors during Visual Search Is Determined by Temporal Task Demands , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[127]  Andrew Hollingworth,et al.  Evidence for negative feature guidance in visual search is explained by spatial recoding. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[128]  Karl J. Friston The free-energy principle: a rough guide to the brain? , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[129]  H. Müller,et al.  Distractor handling via dimension weighting. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[130]  Rufin VanRullen,et al.  On the cyclic nature of perception in vision versus audition , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[131]  Hassana K. Oyibo,et al.  Experience-dependent spatial expectations in mouse visual cortex , 2016, Nature Neuroscience.

[132]  Janneke F. M. Jehee,et al.  Less Is More: Expectation Sharpens Representations in the Primary Visual Cortex , 2012, Neuron.

[133]  Edward Awh,et al.  The role of alpha oscillations in spatial attention: limited evidence for a suppression account. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[134]  L. Squire,et al.  The medial temporal lobe memory system , 1991, Science.

[135]  J. Serences,et al.  Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orienting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[136]  H. Müller,et al.  Feature guidance by negative attentional templates depends on search difficulty , 2019, Visual Cognition.

[137]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Spatial suppression due to statistical regularities is driven by distractor suppression not by target activation , 2018, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[138]  Shih-Cheng Yen,et al.  Mixed selectivity morphs population codes in prefrontal cortex , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[139]  Leonardo Chelazzi,et al.  Orchestrating Proactive and Reactive Mechanisms for Filtering Distracting Information: Brain-Behavior Relationships Revealed by a Mixed-Design fMRI Study , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[140]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Attention, Uncertainty, and Free-Energy , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[141]  Joy J Geng,et al.  Template-to-distractor distinctiveness regulates visual search efficiency. , 2019, Current opinion in psychology.

[142]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Attentional capture during visual search is attenuated by target predictability: evidence from the N2pc, Pd, and topographic segmentation. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[143]  Clayton Hickey,et al.  A temporal dependency account of attentional inhibition in oculomotor control , 2017, NeuroImage.

[144]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Cueing the location of a distractor: an inhibitory mechanism of spatial attention? , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[145]  H. Egeth,et al.  The power of negative thinking: Paradoxical but effective ignoring of salient-but-irrelevant stimuli with a spatial cue , 2018, Visual Cognition.

[146]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Statistical Regularities Modulate Attentional Capture , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[147]  S. Yantis,et al.  Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[148]  Daniel B. Vatterott,et al.  Experience-dependent attentional tuning of distractor rejection , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[149]  O. Jensen,et al.  Shaping Functional Architecture by Oscillatory Alpha Activity: Gating by Inhibition , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[150]  Jason T. Arita,et al.  Templates for rejection: configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[151]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  The lateral intraparietal area as a salience map: the representation of abrupt onset, stimulus motion, and task relevance , 2000, Vision Research.

[152]  Joy J. Geng,et al.  Evidence for Second-Order Singleton Suppression Based on Probabilistic Expectations , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[153]  Floris P de Lange,et al.  Prior expectations induce prestimulus sensory templates , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[154]  M. Eimer The neural basis of attentional control in visual search , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[155]  John J. Foxe,et al.  The Role of Alpha-Band Brain Oscillations as a Sensory Suppression Mechanism during Selective Attention , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[156]  Yuhong V Jiang,et al.  Rapid acquisition but slow extinction of an attentional bias in space. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[157]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A theory of cortical responses , 2005, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[158]  Nancy B. Carlisle,et al.  Distractor Ignoring: Strategies, Learning, and Passive Filtering , 2019, Current directions in psychological science.

[159]  Diane M. Beck,et al.  To See or Not to See: Prestimulus α Phase Predicts Visual Awareness , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[160]  S. Luck,et al.  A Common Neural Mechanism for Preventing and Terminating the Allocation of Attention , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[161]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.