Sensory suppression during shifts of attention between surfaces in transparent motion

During transparent motion, attention to changes in the direction of one illusory surface will impede recognition of a similar event affecting the other surface if both are close together in time. This is a form of object-based attentional blink (AB). Here, we show that this AB is related to a smaller N200 response to the change in direction and that the response is even smaller for trials on which the subject makes mistakes compared to those with correct responses consistent with signal detection theory models. The variation of N200 associated with the AB can be modeled by an attenuation of current sources estimated in visual extrastriate cortex. These results suggest that the AB in the transparent motion paradigm is due to the suppression of sensory signals in early visual areas.

[1]  S. Hillyard,et al.  Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The functional anatomy of attention to visual motion. A functional MRI study. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Similarity determines the attentional blink. , 1995 .

[4]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  Matthias M. Müller,et al.  Effects of spatial selective attention on the steady-state visual evoked potential in the 20-28 Hz range. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[6]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Visual masking and RSVP reveal neural competition , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  R. Andersen,et al.  Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  Kimron Shapiro,et al.  Direct measurement of attentional dwell time in human vision , 1994, Nature.

[9]  M. Scherg,et al.  Intracerebral Sources of Human Auditory-Evoked Potentials , 1999, Audiology and Neurotology.

[10]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[11]  H. Hawkins,et al.  Visual attention modulates signal detectability. , 1990 .

[12]  P. H. Lindsay,et al.  Evoked Potential Correlates of Auditory Signal Detection , 1971, Science.

[13]  C. C. Wood,et al.  The ɛ-Adjustment Procedure for Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance , 1976 .

[14]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Areas Involved in Encoding and Applying Directional Expectations to Moving Objects , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Valdés-Sosa,et al.  Switching Attention without Shifting the Spotlight: Object-Based Attentional Modulation of Brain Potentials , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  M. Valdés-Sosa,et al.  Attention to object files defined by transparent motion. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  H-J Heinze,et al.  Unmasking Motion-Processing Activity in Human Brain Area V5/MT+ Mediated by Pathways That Bypass Primary Visual Cortex , 2002, NeuroImage.

[19]  M Niedeggen,et al.  Characteristics of visual evoked potentials generated by motion coherence onset. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  P. Valdés,et al.  A global scale factor in brain topography. , 1994, The International journal of neuroscience.

[21]  W. Paulus,et al.  Identification of the visual motion area (area V5) in the human brain by dipole source analysis , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  fMRI evidence for objects as the units of attentional selection , 1999, Nature.

[23]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  Jude F. Mitchell,et al.  Object-based attention determines dominance in binocular rivalry , 2004, Nature.

[25]  V. Rodríguez,et al.  El procesamiento del movimiento visual en primates no-humanos (Macaca arctoides) es modulado por la atención basada en objetos , 2006 .

[26]  A. Treisman,et al.  Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.

[27]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Is experimental motion blindness due to sensory suppression? An ERP approach. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  Mitchell Valdes-Sosa,et al.  Attentional shifts between surfaces: effects on detection and early brain potentials , 2001, Vision Research.

[29]  R. Blair,et al.  An alternative method for significance testing of waveform difference potentials. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[30]  E. Vogel,et al.  Word meanings can be accessed but not reported during the attentional blink , 1996, Nature.

[31]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention , 1999, Neuron.

[32]  E. Vogel,et al.  Electrophysiological Evidence for a Postperceptual Locus of Suppression during the Attentional Blink Time-based Attention and the Attentional Blink , 1998 .

[33]  David J. Heeger,et al.  Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  M. Chun,et al.  Types and tokens in visual processing: a double dissociation between the attentional blink and repetition blindness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Alexander M. Harner,et al.  Task-dependent influences of attention on the activation of human primary visual cortex. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[36]  Hiroshi Shibasaki,et al.  Human V5 demonstrated by magnetoencephalography using random dot kinematograms of different coherence levels , 2003, Neuroscience Research.

[37]  Jane E. Raymond,et al.  Attention to visual pattern information produces the attentional blink in rapid serial visual presentation , 1994 .

[38]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[39]  N. Trujillo-Barreto,et al.  3D Statistical Parametric Mapping of EEG Source Spectra by Means of Variable Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (VARETA) , 2001, Clinical EEG.

[40]  R J Ilmoniemi,et al.  Spatiotemporal activity of a cortical network for processing visual motion revealed by MEG and fMRI. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[41]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Area V5 of the human brain: evidence from a combined study using positron emission tomography and magnetic resonance imaging. , 1993, Cerebral cortex.

[42]  M. Valdés-Sosa,et al.  Transparent motion and object-based attention , 1998, Cognition.

[43]  Arash Sahraie,et al.  Attention induced motion blindness , 2001, Vision Research.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A direct quantitative relationship between the functional properties of human and macaque V5 , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  M. Meng,et al.  Relationship between ventral stream for object vision and dorsal stream for spatial vision: An fMRI+ERP study , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[46]  R. Biscay,et al.  Testing topographic differences between event related brain potentials by using non-parametric combinations of permutation tests. , 1997, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[47]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[48]  W. Newsome,et al.  Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.