Glutamate infused posttraining into the hippocampus or caudate-putamen differentially strengthens place and response learning.

A cross-maze task that can be acquired through either place or response learning was used to examine the hypothesis that posttraining neurochemical manipulation of the hippocampus or caudate-putamen can bias an animal toward the use of a specific memory system. Male Long-Evans rats received four trials per day for 7 days, a probe trial on day 8, further training on days 9-15, and an additional probe trial on day 16. Training occurred in a cross-maze task in which rats started from a consistent start-box (south), and obtained food from a consistent goal-arm (west). On days 4-6 of training, rats received posttraining intrahippocampal (1 microgram/0.5 microliter) or intracaudate (2 microgram/0.5 microliter) injections of either glutamate or saline (0.5 microliter). On days 8 and 16, a probe trial was given in which rats were placed in a novel start-box (north). Rats selecting the west goal-arm were designated "place" learners, and those selecting the east goal-arm were designated "response" learners. Saline-treated rats predominantly displayed place learning on day 8 and response learning on day 16, indicating a shift in control of learned behavior with extended training. Rats receiving intrahippocampal injections of glutamate predominantly displayed place learning on days 8 and 16, indicating that manipulation of the hippocampus produced a blockade of the shift to response learning. Rats receiving intracaudate injections of glutamate displayed response learning on days 8 and 16, indicating an accelerated shift to response learning. The findings suggest that posttraining intracerebral glutamate infusions can (i) modulate the distinct memory processes mediated by the hippocampus and caudate-putamen and (ii) bias the brain toward the use of a specific memory system to control learned behavior and thereby influence the timing of the switch from the use of cognitive memory to habit learning to guide behavior.

[1]  N. White,et al.  Parallel Information Processing in the Dorsal Striatum: Relation to Hippocampal Function , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Packard,et al.  Dissociation of multiple memory systems by posttraining intracerebral injections of glutamate , 1999, Psychobiology.

[3]  S. Charpier,et al.  In vivo activity-dependent plasticity at cortico-striatal connections: evidence for physiological long-term potentiation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  M. Packard,et al.  Double dissociation of hippocampal and dorsal-striatal memory systems by posttraining intracerebral injections of 2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoic acid. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  A Neostriatal Habit Learning System in Humans , 1996, Science.

[6]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Inactivation of Hippocampus or Caudate Nucleus with Lidocaine Differentially Affects Expression of Place and Response Learning , 1996, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[7]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Amygdala modulation of hippocampal-dependent and caudate nucleus-dependent memory processes. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  R. J. McDonald,et al.  Parallel information processing in the water maze: evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampus. , 1994, Behavioral and neural biology.

[9]  T. Bliss,et al.  A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus , 1993, Nature.

[10]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system , 1993 .

[11]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Long-term synaptic depression in the striatum: physiological and pharmacological characterization , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[12]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Long‐term Potentiation in the Striatum is Unmasked by Removing the Voltage‐dependent Magnesium Block of NMDA Receptor Channels , 1992, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Neurotransmitter receptors involved in post-training memory processing by the amygdala, medial septum, and hippocampus of the rat. , 1992, Behavioral and neural biology.

[14]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Double dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: further evidence for multiple memory systems. , 1992, Behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  S. Young,et al.  Presynaptic long‐term changes in excitability of the corticostriatal pathway , 1992, Neuroreport.

[16]  M. Packard,et al.  Dissociation of hippocampus and caudate nucleus memory systems by posttraining intracerebral injection of dopamine agonists , 1991 .

[17]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Modulation of memory processing by glutamic acid receptor agonists and antagonists , 1990, Brain Research.

[18]  R. Sutherland,et al.  Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia , 1989, Psychobiology.

[19]  M. Packard,et al.  Differential effects of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on two radial maze tasks: evidence for multiple memory systems , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[20]  N. White,et al.  Dissociation of visual and olfactory conditioning in the neostriatum of rats , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[21]  V. Chan‐Palay,et al.  The Hippocampus : new vistas , 1989 .

[22]  Jane A. Mitchell,et al.  Caudate-Putamen Lesions in the Rat may Impair or Potentiate Maze Learning Depending upon Availability of Stimulus Cues and Relevance of Response Cues , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[23]  R. Kesner,et al.  Caudate nucleus and memory for egocentric localization. , 1988, Behavioral and neural biology.

[24]  D. Salmon,et al.  Impaired learning of a motor skill in patients with Huntington's disease. , 1988, Behavioral neuroscience.

[25]  N. White Effect of nigrostriatal dopamine depletion on the post-training, memory-improving action of amphetamine. , 1988, Life sciences.

[26]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Evolution of Multiple Memory Systems , 1987 .

[27]  D. Schacter,et al.  Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[28]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Monkeys with combined amygdalo-hippocampal lesions succeed in object discrimination learning despite 24-hour intertrial intervals. , 1984, Behavioral neuroscience.

[29]  J. Becker,et al.  Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia. , 1984, Archives of neurology.

[30]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning in monkeys with medial temporal lesions: sparing of motor and cognitive skills , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[31]  G. Paxinos,et al.  The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates , 1983 .

[32]  M. Mishkin,et al.  The neuroanatomy of amnesia: amygdala-hippocampus versus temporal stem. , 1982, Science.

[33]  I. Divac,et al.  Biochemical evidence for glutamate as neurotransmitter in corticostriatal and corticothalamic fibres in rat brain , 1981, Neuroscience.

[34]  W. Gary Thompson,et al.  Effects of caudate and cortical lesions on place and response learning in rats , 1980 .

[35]  L. Squire,et al.  Preserved learning and retention of pattern-analyzing skill in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. , 1980, Science.

[36]  R. Passingham The hippocampus as a cognitive map J. O'Keefe & L. Nadel, Oxford University Press, Oxford (1978). 570 pp., £25.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[37]  L. Nadel,et al.  The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map , 1978 .

[38]  R. Hirsh The hippocampus and contextual retrieval of information from memory: a theory. , 1974, Behavioral biology.

[39]  J. D. Castro A selective spatial discrimination deficit after fornicotomy in the rat. , 1974 .

[40]  A. A. Buerger,et al.  Effects of ventral putamen lesions on discrimination learning by monkeys. , 1974, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[41]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Behavioral effects of selective ablation of the caudate nucleus. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[42]  J L McGaugh,et al.  Time-Dependent Processes in Memory Storage , 1966, Science.

[43]  L. H. Hicks Effects of Overtraining on Acquisition and Reversal of Place and Response Learning , 1964 .

[44]  F. Restle Discrimination of cues in mazes: a resolution of the place-vs.-response question. , 1957, Psychological review.

[45]  B. Ritchie,et al.  Studies in spatial learning; place performance and the acquisition of place dispositions. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[46]  E. Tolman There is more than one kind of learning. , 1949, Psychological review.

[47]  C. P. Duncan,et al.  The retroactive effect of electroshock on learning. , 1949, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[48]  E. Tolman,et al.  Studies in spatial learning; place learning versus response learning. , 1946, Journal of experimental psychology.

[49]  E. Thorndike A PROOF OF THE LAW OF EFFECT. , 1933, Science.

[50]  F. W. Irwin Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men , 1932, The Psychological Clinic.

[51]  E. Tolman Purposive behavior in animals and men , 1932 .