Opinions on the Tax Deductibility of Mortgages and the Consensus Effect

SummaryWe present the results of a survey-experiment – using a representative sample of the Dutch population – in which we relate respondents’ opinion about a restriction of the tax deductibility of mortgages to their estimates about other people’s opinions. We find a strong consensus effect; meaning that respondents’ estimates of others’ opinions are related to their own opinion. Furthermore, we find that the size of the effect is not affected by the ambiguity of the question posed. The provision of arguments pro and contra the tax provision and monetary incentives for accuracy reduce the consensus effect, but only so in conjunction. Finally, we find that house owners display a significantly stronger consensus effect. Our results suggest that both cognitive and motivational factors are responsible for the consensus effect. Aside from the consensus effect, our survey gives interesting insights into people’s opinion on tax deductibility of mortgages. A majority consider a general restriction to be unfair, but a proposal to restrict only mortgages as of a certain size meets with much more approval.

[1]  Colin Camerer Individual Decision Making , 2020, The Handbook of Experimental Economics.

[2]  A. Manstead Perceived social support for opinions: A test of the magnitude and diversity hypotheses , 1982 .

[3]  Martin Strobel,et al.  The False Consensus Effect: Deconstruction and Reconstruction of an Anomaly , 2004 .

[4]  Dirk Engelmann,et al.  The False Consensus Effect Disappears if Representative Information and Monetary Incentives Are Given , 2000 .

[5]  Clifford E. Brown A False Consensus Bias in 1980 Presidential PReferences , 1982 .

[6]  J. Kagel,et al.  Handbook of Experimental Economics , 1997 .

[7]  L. Ross,et al.  The “false consensus effect”: An egocentric bias in social perception and attribution processes , 1977 .

[8]  Axel Ockenfels,et al.  An experimental solidarity game , 1998 .

[9]  J. Sonnemans,et al.  Value Orientations, Expectations and Voluntary Contributions in Public Goods. , 1996 .

[10]  Robyn M. Dawes,et al.  The potential nonfalsity of the false consensus effect. , 1990 .

[11]  B. Mullen,et al.  Social projection as a function of cognitive mechanisms: Two meta‐analytic integrations , 1988 .

[12]  Robert Forsythe,et al.  Anatomy of an Experimental Political Stock Market , 1992 .

[13]  W. Crano Assumed Consensus of Attitudes , 1983 .

[14]  G. Goethals,et al.  Perceptions of the magnitude and diversity of social support , 1979 .

[15]  Ian Walker,et al.  The Returns to Education: Microeconomics , 2003 .

[16]  Norman Miller,et al.  Ten years of research on the false-consensus effect: an empirical and theoretical review , 1987 .

[17]  G. Charness,et al.  Relative Payoffs and Happiness: An Experimental Study , 2001 .

[18]  Abdolkarim Sadrieh,et al.  Experimental Proof for the Motivational Importance of Reciprocity , 1996 .

[19]  Chun-Lei Yang,et al.  The Hot Versus Cold Effect in a Simple Bargaining Experiment , 2003 .

[20]  J. D. Campbell Similarity and uniqueness: the effects of attribute type, relevance, and individual differences in self-esteem and depression. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.