The Malicious Serpent

As reptiles, snakes may have signified deadly threats in the environment of early mammals. We review findings suggesting that snakes remain special stimuli for humans. Intense snake fear is prevalent in both humans and other primates. Humans and monkeys learn snake fear more easily than fear of most other stimuli through direct or vicarious conditioning. Neither the elicitation nor the conditioning of snake fear in humans requires that snakes be consciously perceived; rather, both processes can occur with masked stimuli. Humans tend to perceive illusory correlations between snakes and aversive stimuli, and their attention is automatically captured by snakes in complex visual displays. Together, these and other findings delineate an evolved fear module in the brain. This module is selectively and automatically activated by once-threatening stimuli, is relatively encapsulated from cognition, and derives from specialized neural circuitry.

[1]  C. Sagan The Dragons of Eden: Speculations on the Evolution of Human Intelligence , 1977 .

[2]  S. Mineka Evolutionary Memories, Emotional Processing, and the Emotional Disorders , 1992 .

[3]  B. Kolb,et al.  Cortical Plasticity and the Development of Behavior After Early Frontal Cortical Injury , 2000, Developmental neuropsychology.

[4]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fear-relevant illusory correlations: what types of associations promote judgmental bias? , 1995, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[5]  S. Mineka,et al.  Selective associations in the origins of phobic fears and their implications for behavior therapy. , 1991 .

[6]  B. Kolb,et al.  Age, Experience and the Changing Brain , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[7]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[8]  R. Rapee,et al.  Covariation bias for phylogenetic versus ontogenetic fear-relevant stimuli. , 1997, Behaviour research and therapy.

[9]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fear of snakes in wild- and laboratory-reared rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) , 1980 .

[12]  David A. Sylvester,et al.  The epidemiology of common fears and phobia. , 1969, Comprehensive psychiatry.

[13]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[14]  R. Bootzin,et al.  Theoretical Issues in Behavior Therapy , 1985 .

[15]  S. Mineka,et al.  Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[16]  A. Ohman,et al.  "Unconscious anxiety": phobic responses to masked stimuli. , 1994, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[17]  Stefan Wiens,et al.  Unawareness is more than a chance event: comment on Lovibond and Shanks (2002). , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[18]  A. Ohman,et al.  On the automatic nature of phobic fear: conditioned electrodermal responses to masked fear-relevant stimuli. , 1993, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  Paul R. Martin Handbook of behavior therapy and psychological science : an integrative approach , 1991 .

[20]  P. Lang,et al.  Preparedness and phobia: effects of stimulus content on human visceral conditioning. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.