Predator stimuli and calling behavior of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis), tufted titmice (Baeolophus bicolor), and white-breasted nuthatches (Sitta carolinensis)
暂无分享,去创建一个
Todd M. Freeberg | Carrie L. Branch | T. Freeberg | C. Branch | Julia Bartmess-LeVasseur | Sheri A. Browning | Jessica L. Owens | Julia N. Bartmess-Levasseur
[1] Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al. CHAPTER 13 “ Information ” and the chickadee call : Communicating with a complex vocal system , 2007 .
[2] Leonard A. Brennan,et al. Ecology and Behavior of Chickadees and Titmice: An Integrated Approach , 2007 .
[3] C. Peres,et al. Anti-predation benefits in a mixed-species group of Amazonian tamarins. , 1993, Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology.
[4] H. Papoušek,et al. Nonverbal Vocal Communication: Comparative and Developmental Approaches , 2008 .
[5] S. L. Lima,et al. Back to the basics of anti-predatory vigilance: the group-size effect , 1995, Animal Behaviour.
[6] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Chick-a-dee call variation in Carolina chickadees and recruiting flockmates to food , 2009 .
[7] Etienne Sirot,et al. Social information, antipredatory vigilance and flight in bird flocks , 2006, Animal Behaviour.
[8] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Complexity in the Chick-a-Dee Call of Carolina Chickadees (Poecile Carolinensis): Associations of Context and Signaler Behavior to Call Structure , 2008 .
[9] E. Curio,et al. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing: I. Teleonomic hypotheses and predictions. , 1978 .
[10] K. Sullivan. Information Exploitation By Downy Woodpeckers in Mixed-Species Flocks , 1984 .
[11] U. Ernst,et al. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing: II. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: Effectiveness and some constraints. , 2010 .
[12] S. Haftorn. CONTEXTS AND POSSIBLE FUNCTIONS OF ALARM CALLING IN THE WILLOW TIT, PARUS MONTANUS; THE PRINCIPLE OF ‘BETTER SAFE THAN SORRY’ , 2000 .
[13] F. James Rohlf,et al. Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .
[14] I. Krams,et al. Effects of Cover on Loud Trill-Call and Soft Seet-Call Use in the Crested tit Parus Cristatus , 2008 .
[15] E. Morton. On the Occurrence and Significance of Motivation-Structural Rules in Some Bird and Mammal Sounds , 1977, The American Naturalist.
[16] Susan M. Smith. The Black-Capped Chickadee: Behavioral Ecology and Natural History , 1992 .
[17] T. Freeberg. Social Complexity Can Drive Vocal Complexity , 2006, Psychological science.
[18] J. Hailman,et al. The ‘chick-a-dee’ calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English , 1985 .
[19] D. H. Morse,et al. Ecological Aspects of Some Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks of Birds , 1970 .
[20] Alison S. Garton. Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches , 1993 .
[21] D. Kroodsma,et al. Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds , 1997 .
[22] P. Marler. Chapter 5 – Bird calls: a cornucopia for communication , 2004 .
[23] C. E. Ho,et al. A procedure for an automated measurement of song similarity , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[24] Philip Gaddis,et al. A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF THE VOCAL COMMUNICATION SYSTEMS OF THE CAROLINA CHICKADEE AND THE TUFTED TITMOUSE , 1979 .
[25] Robin I. M. Dunbar. Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language , 1996 .
[26] Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al. Chickadee vocal production and perception: An integrative approach to understanding acoustic communication , 2007 .
[27] P. Marler,et al. Nature's Music: The Science of Birdsong , 2004 .
[28] Jack W. Bradbury,et al. Principles of Animal Communication , 1998 .
[29] E. Frankenberg,et al. The Adaptive Significance of Avian Mobbing , 2010 .
[30] R. R. Krausz. Living in Groups , 2013 .
[31] Erick Greene,et al. Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[32] P. Bell. Darwin's biological work , 1960 .
[33] April M. Becker,et al. MOBBING CALLS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES: EFFECTS OF URGENCY ON CALL PRODUCTION , 2002 .
[34] J. P. Hailman,et al. The organization of major vocalizations in the paridae , 1989 .
[35] Wooseok Lee,et al. Novel acoustic features for speech emotion recognition , 2009 .
[36] Jessica L Owens,et al. Variation in chick-a-dee calls of tufted titmice, Baeolophus bicolor: note type and individual distinctiveness. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[37] G. Ritchison. Vocalizations of the white-breasted nuthatch , 1983 .
[38] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[39] Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al. Asymmetries in mobbing behaviour and correlated intensity during predator mobbing by nuthatches, chickadees and titmice , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[40] J. R. Landis,et al. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. , 1977, Biometrics.
[41] Gerhard Manteuffel,et al. Castration-induced vocalisation in domestic piglets, Sus scrofa: Complex and specific alterations of the vocal quality , 2005 .
[42] K. Wells. Animal Vocal Communication: A New Approach , 1999 .
[43] Jack P. Hailman,et al. The chick-a-dee call system of the Mexican chickadee , 1994 .
[44] J. Ekman. ECOLOGY OF NON-BREEDING SOCIAL SYSTEMS OF PARUS , 1989 .
[45] Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al. Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. III: The chick-a-dee call of the Carolina chickadee (Poecile carolinensis) , 2005 .
[46] L. Sasvári. Great tits benefit from feeding in mixed-species flocks: a field experiment , 1992, Animal Behaviour.
[47] Guy Cowlishaw,et al. When to use social information: the advantage of large group size in individual decision making , 2007, Biology Letters.