V1 neurons encode the perceptual compensation of false torsion arising from Listing’s law

Significance Eye movements have three degrees of freedom, the ability to move horizontally, vertically, and torsionally. Eye movements serving the fovea strictly control eye torsion. The amount of torsion associated with a particular eye position is specified by Listing’s law. Because of prior knowledge of the torsion set by Listing’s law, we perceive the world as upright although its retinal image may be tilted by varying degrees. We first show that also monkeys use a torsion prior to ensure a veridical perceptual reinterpretation of image tilt. We then demonstrate that this reinterpretation is a consequence of the fact that V1 neurons use the torsion prior to adjust their orientation preferences and to reposition their receptive fields relative to the retina. We try to deploy the retinal fovea to optimally scrutinize an object of interest by directing our eyes to it. The horizontal and vertical components of eye positions acquired by goal-directed saccades are determined by the object’s location. However, the eccentric eye positions also involve a torsional component, which according to Donder’s law is fully determined by the two-dimensional (2D) eye position acquired. According to von Helmholtz, knowledge of the amount of torsion provided by Listing’s law, an extension of Donder’s law, alleviates the perceptual interpretation of the image tilt that changes with 2D eye position, a view supported by psychophysical experiments he pioneered. We address the question of where and how Listing’s law is implemented in the visual system and we show that neurons in monkey area V1 use knowledge of eye torsion to compensate the image tilt associated with specific eye positions as set by Listing’s law.

[1]  H. Helmholtz Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 2015 .

[2]  Mingsha Zhang,et al.  The proprioceptive representation of eye position in monkey primary somatosensory cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  K. Hepp,et al.  Smooth pursuit eye movements obey Listing's law in the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  W. Haustein,et al.  Evaluation of retinal orientation and gaze direction in the perception of the vertical , 1990, Vision Research.

[5]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Listing's law, eye position sense, and perception of the vertical , 1977, Vision Research.

[6]  U. Büttner,et al.  Deficits in vertical and torsional eye movements after uni- and bilateral muscimol inactivation of the interstitial nucleus of Cajal of the alert monkey , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  K Hepp,et al.  Role of Monkey Nucleus Reticularis Tegmenti Pontis in the Stabilization of Listing’s Plane , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Peter Thier,et al.  Monkeys use the rod-dense retinal region rather than the fovea to visually fixate small targets in scotopic vision , 2018, bioRxiv.

[9]  Hongying Wang,et al.  Three-dimensional eye-head coordination is implemented downstream from the superior colliculus. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  T Vilis,et al.  Symmetry of oculomotor burst neuron coordinates about Listing's plane. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Y. Benjamini,et al.  Resampling-based false discovery rate controlling multiple test procedures for correlated test statistics , 1999 .

[12]  T. Bonhoeffer,et al.  Overrepresentation of horizontal and vertical orientation preferences in developing ferret area 17. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[13]  K. Hepp,et al.  On the generation of vertical and torsional rapid eye movements in the monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Jaime de la Rocha,et al.  Supplementary Information for the article ‘ Correlation between neural spike trains increases with firing rate ’ , 2007 .

[15]  U. Büttner,et al.  Saccade-related burst neurons with torsional and vertical on-directions in the interstitial nucleus of Cajal of the alert monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  T. Vilis,et al.  Computing three-dimensional eye position quaternions and eye velocity from search coil signals , 1990, Vision Research.

[17]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  Ian S. Curthoys,et al.  On the relation between ocular torsion and visual perception of line orientation , 2008, Vision Research.

[19]  J. Troendle,et al.  Stepwise normal theory multiple test procedures controlling the false discovery rate , 2000 .

[20]  C. Acuña,et al.  Discrimination of line orientation in humans and monkeys. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  P. Thier,et al.  Eye position information is used to compensate the consequences of ocular torsion on V1 receptive fields , 2014, Nature Communications.

[22]  Bernhard J M Hess,et al.  Three-dimensional visuo-motor control of saccades. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Kinematics of the eye. , 1957, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[24]  D. Snodderly,et al.  Organization of striate cortex of alert, trained monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): ongoing activity, stimulus selectivity, and widths of receptive field activating regions. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  A. V. van den Berg,et al.  Perceptual compensation for eye torsion , 2005, Vision Research.