Using a Dynamic Neural Field Model to Explore a Direct Collicular Inhibition Account of Inhibition of Return

When the interval between a transient ash of light (a "cue") and a second visual response signal (a "target") exceeds at least 200ms, responding is slowest in the direction indicated by the first signal. This phenomenon is commonly referred to as inhibition of return (IOR). The dynamic neural field model (DNF) has proven to have broad explanatory power for IOR, effectively capturing many empirical results. Previous work has used a short-term depression (STD) implementation of IOR, but this approach fails to explain many behavioral phenomena observed in the literature. Here, we explore a variant model of IOR involving a combination of STD and delayed direct collicular inhibition. We demonstrate that this hybrid model can better reproduce established behavioural results. We use the results of this model to propose several experiments that would yield particularly valuable insight into the nature of the neurophysiological mechanisms underlying IOR.

[1]  E. Maylor,et al.  Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  A. Sereno,et al.  Disruption of Reflexive Attention and Eye Movements in an Individual with a Collicular Lesion , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  Matthew D. Hilchey,et al.  Averaging saccades are repelled by prior uninformative cues at both short and long intervals , 2012 .

[4]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .

[5]  R. Klein,et al.  Contribution of the Primate Superior Colliculus to Inhibition of Return , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Matthew D. Hilchey,et al.  On the nature of the delayed “inhibitory” Cueing effects generated by uninformative arrows at fixation , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[7]  Matthew D. Hilchey,et al.  Oculomotor inhibition of return: How soon is it “recoded” into spatiotopic coordinates? , 2012, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[8]  C. Kennard,et al.  Distinct Cortical and Collicular Mechanisms of Inhibition of Return Revealed with S Cone Stimuli , 2004, Current Biology.

[9]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  The time course of saccadic decision making: Dynamic field theory , 2006, Neural Networks.

[10]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Inhibition of return in spatial attention: direct evidence for collicular generation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  PERCEPTUAL AND MOTOR IOR: COMPONENTS OR FLAVOURS? , 2011 .

[13]  R. Segev,et al.  Inhibition of return in the archer fish , 2013, Nature Communications.

[14]  Kristie R. Dukewich Reconceptualizing inhibition of return as , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Object-based inhibition of return in static displays , 1998 .

[16]  R. Klein,et al.  A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  S. Tipper,et al.  Short Report: Object-Centred Inhibition of Return of Visual Attention , 1991, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[18]  R. Klein,et al.  Aftereffects of Saccades Explored in a Dynamic Neural Field Model of the Superior Colliculus , 2011 .

[19]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Effects of parietal lesions on visual matching: Implications for reading errors , 1985 .

[20]  J. Cowan,et al.  A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue , 1973, Kybernetik.

[21]  A. Kingstone,et al.  Unmasking the inhibition of return phenomenon , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  R. Klein,et al.  Modeling inhibition of return as short-term depression of early sensory input to the superior colliculus , 2011, Vision Research.

[23]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of Return is a Foraging Facilitator in Visual Search , 1999 .

[24]  Luis J Fuentes,et al.  Inhibitory processing following damage to the parietal lobe , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  Sensory and motor mechanisms of oculomotor inhibition of return , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Parietal Lobe Lesions Disrupt Saccadic Remapping of Inhibitory Location Tagging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  S. Amari Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields , 1977, Biological Cybernetics.

[28]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Hemispheric Asymmetry in the Remapping and Maintenance of Visual Saliency Maps: A TMS Study , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  R. Klein,et al.  Searching for inhibition of return in visual search: A review , 2010, Vision Research.

[30]  Andrew R. Mayer,et al.  An Event-related fMRI Study of Exogenous Orienting: Supporting Evidence for the Cortical Basis of Inhibition of Return? , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  A. Samuel,et al.  Inhibition of return: A graphical meta-analysis of its time course and an empirical test of its temporal and spatial properties , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  G. Tassinari,et al.  Sensory and attentional components of slowing of manual reaction time to non-fixated visual targets by ipsilateral primes , 1993, Vision Research.

[34]  Gregor Schöner,et al.  Saccadic motor planning by integrating visual information and pre-information on neural dynamic fields , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[35]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Correlates of Capture of Attention and Inhibition of Return across Stages of Visual Processing , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Eitaro Aiyoshi,et al.  A distributed model of the saccade system: simulations of temporally perturbed saccades using position and velocity feedback , 1999, Neural Networks.

[37]  J. Bisley,et al.  Been there, seen that: a neural mechanism for performing efficient visual search. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  S P Tipper,et al.  Object-based facilitation and inhibition from visual orienting in the human split-brain. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  Jonathan Vaughan,et al.  Saccades Directed at Previously Attended Locations in Space , 1984 .

[41]  Raymond Klein,et al.  Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search , 1988, Nature.

[42]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Cortical expressions of inhibition of return , 2006, Brain Research.

[43]  Michel Thiebaut de Schotten,et al.  Cortical control of inhibition of return: Evidence from patients with inferior parietal damage and visual neglect , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Jillian H. Fecteau,et al.  Vying for dominance: dynamic interactions control visual fixation and saccadic initiation in the superior colliculus. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[45]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[46]  Chris Rorden,et al.  Saccade preparation is required for exogenous attention but not endogenous attention or IOR. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.