The swaying behavior of Extatosoma tiaratum: motion camouflage in a stick insect?
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H. B. Cott,et al. Adaptive Coloration in Animals , 1940 .
[2] A. B. Gurney. Notes on Some Remarkable Australasian Walkingsticks, Including a Synopsis of the Genus Extatosoma (Orthoptera: Phasmatidae) , 1947 .
[3] Kenneth S. Norris,et al. An Analysis of Background Color‐Matching in Amphibians and Reptiles , 1964 .
[4] J. Kennedy. Notes on the Habitat and Behavior of a Snake, Oxybelis aeneus Wagler, in Veracruz , 1965 .
[5] J. Readshaw. A theory of phasmatid outbreak release. , 1965 .
[6] H. Heatwole,et al. Relationship of Escape Behavior and Camouflage in Anoline Lizards , 1968 .
[7] M. Edmunds,et al. Defence in Animals , 1976 .
[8] P. Maclean,et al. Behavior and neurology of lizards : an interdisciplinary colloquium / , 1978 .
[9] J. Endler. A Predator’s View of Animal Color Patterns , 1978 .
[10] J. Endler. An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis , 1981 .
[11] J. Endler. Progressive background in moths, and a quantitative measure of crypsis , 1984 .
[12] D. Regan,et al. Figure-ground segregation by motion contrast and by luminance contrast. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[13] Leo J. Fleishman,et al. Cryptic movement in the vine snake Oxybelis aeneus , 1985 .
[14] Leo J. Fleishman,et al. Sensory influences on physical design of a visual display , 1988, Animal Behaviour.
[15] U. Bässler. Functional principles of pattern generation for walking movements of stick insect forelegs: the role of the femoral chordotonal organ afferences , 1988 .
[16] P. Hannah,et al. Wind and Trees: Predicting windspeeds for forest areas in complex terrain , 1995 .
[17] T. Koga,et al. Burrow defense behaviors in a sand-bubbler crab, Scopimera globosa, in relation to body size and prior residence , 2001, Journal of Ethology.
[18] U. Bässler,et al. The control-system of the femur-tibia-joint of the phasmidExtatosoma tiaratum and the control of rocking , 1979, Journal of comparative physiology.
[19] U. Carlberg. Defensive secretion of stick insects , 1981, Journal of Chemical Ecology.
[20] L. Fleishman. Motion detection in the presence and absence of background motion in anAnolis lizard , 1986, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[21] Sami Merilaita,et al. Background-matching and disruptive coloration, and the evolution of cryptic coloration , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[22] D. Eilam. Die hard: A blend of freezing and fleeing as a dynamic defense—implications for the control of defensive behavior , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[23] Mark Broom,et al. You can run--or you can hide: optimal strategies for cryptic prey against pursuit predators , 2005 .
[24] R. Peters,et al. Signaling against the Wind: Modifying Motion-Signal Structure in Response to Increased Noise , 2007, Current Biology.
[25] Yixin Zhang,et al. Unidirectional prey–predator facilitation: apparent prey enhance predators' foraging success on cryptic prey , 2007, Biology Letters.
[26] J. Godin,et al. Behaviourally mediated crypsis in two nocturnal moths with contrasting appearance , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[27] Tyson L Hedrick,et al. Software techniques for two- and three-dimensional kinematic measurements of biological and biomimetic systems , 2008, Bioinspiration & biomimetics.
[28] S. Merilaita,et al. Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[29] A. T. Dossey,et al. Developmental and Geographical Variation in the Chemical Defense of the Walkingstick Insect Anisomorpha buprestoides , 2008, Journal of Chemical Ecology.
[30] S. Merilaita,et al. Concealed by conspicuousness: distractive prey markings and backgrounds , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[31] C. Ioannou,et al. Interactions between background matching and motion during visual detection can explain why cryptic animals keep still , 2009, Biology Letters.
[32] Leo J. Fleishman,et al. Modeling and measuring the visual detection of ecologically relevant motion by an Anolis lizard , 2009, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.
[33] Tim Guilford. Predator Psychology and the Evolution of Prey Coloration , 2009 .
[34] Hannah M. Rowland,et al. The evolution and ecology of masquerade , 2009 .
[35] P. D. Brock,et al. The Complete Field Guide to Stick and Leaf Insects of Australia , 2009 .
[36] M. H. Robinson. The defensive behaviour of some orthopteroid insects from Panama , 2009 .
[37] E. Poulton. The colours of animals: Their meaning and use: Especially considered in the case of insects , 2009 .
[38] M. Elgar,et al. Facultative sex and reproductive strategies in response to male availability in the spiny stick insect, Extatosoma tiaratum , 2010 .
[39] G. Ruxton,et al. Predators are less likely to misclassify masquerading prey when their models are present , 2010, Biology Letters.
[40] N. Tinbergen. On aims and methods of Ethology , 2010 .
[41] Graeme D Ruxton,et al. Masquerade: Camouflage Without Crypsis , 2010, Science.
[42] Y. Stuart,et al. Inferring Predator Behavior from Attack Rates on Prey-Replicas That Differ in Conspicuousness , 2012, PloS one.
[43] Sang-im Lee,et al. Cryptically Patterned Moths Perceive Bark Structure When Choosing Body Orientations That Match Wing Color Pattern to the Bark Pattern , 2013, PloS one.
[44] R Core Team,et al. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .
[45] P. Brockhoff,et al. lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package) , 2014 .
[46] S. Blamires,et al. Evidence of bird dropping masquerading by a spider to avoid predators , 2014, Scientific Reports.
[47] D. Bates,et al. Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .