Just another face in the crowd: Evidence for decreased detection of angry faces in children with Williams syndrome

The detection of social threat is crucial for adaptive behaviour. Previous studies have shown that angry faces capture attention and are processed more efficiently than happy faces. While this anger superiority effect has been found in typical and atypical development, it is unknown whether it exists in individuals with Williams syndrome (WS), who show reduced social fear and atypical sociability. In this study, children with WS searched for angry or happy target faces surrounded by 2, 5 or 8 distracters (happy or angry faces, respectively). Performance was compared to that of mental age-matched controls. Results revealed no group differences for happy faces, however for angry faces, the WS, but not the control group, showed a significant performance decrease for the 8-distracters condition, indicating the absence of an anger superiority effect, in good agreement with evidence for abnormal structure and function in brain areas for social threat processing in WS.

[1]  Vanessa Lobue,et al.  More than just another face in the crowd: superior detection of threatening facial expressions in children and adults. , 2009, Developmental science.

[2]  A. Ohman,et al.  The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  C. Deruelle,et al.  Global and Local processing in Williams Syndrome: Drawing versus Perceiving , 2008, Child neuropsychology : a journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence.

[4]  C. Jarrold,et al.  Visuospatial Cognition in Williams Syndrome: Reviewing and Accounting for the Strengths and Weaknesses in Performance , 2003, Developmental neuropsychology.

[5]  C. Deruelle,et al.  Exploring face processing in Williams syndrome. , 2003 .

[6]  Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Genetic Contributions to Human Gyrification: Sulcal Morphometry in Williams Syndrome , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  C. Mervis,et al.  Global Spatial Organization by Individuals with Williams Syndrome , 1999 .

[8]  C. Mervis,et al.  A genetic model for understanding higher order visual processing: functional interactions of the ventral visual stream in Williams syndrome. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Francisco Esteves,et al.  The face of wrath: The role of features and configurations in conveying social threat , 2004, Cognition & emotion.

[10]  A. Calder Facial Emotion Recognition after Bilateral Amygdala Damage: Differentially Severe Impairment of Fear , 1996 .

[11]  P. Hancock,et al.  Do Faces Capture the Attention of Individuals with Williams Syndrome or Autism? Evidence from Tracking Eye Movements , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[12]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  Reduced parietal and visual cortical activation during global processing in Williams syndrome , 2007, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[13]  E. Fox,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: Are Angry Faces Detected More Efficiently? , 2000, Cognition & emotion.

[14]  E. Bizzi,et al.  The Cognitive Neurosciences , 1996 .

[15]  Francisco Esteves,et al.  The Face of Wrath: Critical Features for Conveying Facial Threat , 1999 .

[16]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Fear and the human amygdala , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[17]  Jon Driver,et al.  Visual search in typically developing toddlers and toddlers with Fragile X or Williams syndrome. , 2004, Developmental science.

[18]  J. Stiles-Davis,et al.  Spatial cognition : brain bases and development , 1988 .

[19]  H. Critchley,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline , 1991, Cognition.

[21]  Y. Bar-Haim,et al.  Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  C. Deruelle,et al.  Human Versus Non-Human Face Processing: Evidence from Williams Syndrome , 2009, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[23]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[24]  R. Schultz Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area , 2005, International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience.

[25]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Visual search for faces with emotional expressions. , 2008, Psychological bulletin.

[26]  C. Mervis,et al.  Retinotopically defined primary visual cortex in Williams syndrome. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  G. Horstmann,et al.  Search asymmetries with real faces: testing the anger-superiority effect. , 2006, Emotion.

[29]  M. Mather,et al.  Angry faces get noticed quickly: threat detection is not impaired among older adults. , 2006, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[30]  C. Deruelle,et al.  Configural and Local Processing of Faces in Children with Williams Syndrome , 1999, Brain and Cognition.

[31]  D. McIntosh,et al.  Rapid facial reactions to emotional facial expressions in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorder. , 2008, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[32]  S. Rauch,et al.  A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. , 2001, Emotion.

[33]  W. Yule,et al.  Cognitive abilities and behavioural characteristics of children with idiopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. , 1987, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[34]  R. Blair,et al.  Facial expressions, their communicatory functions and neuro-cognitive substrates. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[35]  John Patrick Aggleton,et al.  The Amygdala : a functional analysis , 2000 .

[36]  D. Perrett,et al.  A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions , 1996, Nature.

[37]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  Susan Faja,et al.  Perceiving facial and vocal expressions of emotion in individuals with Williams syndrome. , 2006, American journal of mental retardation : AJMR.

[39]  David I. Perrett,et al.  Facial expression recognition in Williams syndrome , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Daniel Smilek,et al.  Visual search is not blind to emotion , 2008, Perception & psychophysics.

[41]  D. Perrett,et al.  Is everybody always my friend? Perception of approachability in Williams syndrome , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  R. Adolphs Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[43]  J. Grèzes,et al.  A failure to grasp the affective meaning of actions in autism spectrum disorder subjects , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  Svein Magnussen,et al.  Patterns of Perceptual Asymmetry in Processing Facial Expression , 1994, Cortex.

[45]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Emotional facial expressions capture attention , 2001, Neurology.

[46]  A. Young,et al.  Understanding face recognition. , 1986, British journal of psychology.

[47]  Joseph E. LeDoux,et al.  How danger is encoded: towards a systems, cellular, and computational understanding of cognitive-emotional interactions , 1999 .

[48]  Ralph Adolphs,et al.  Fear, faces, and the human amygdala , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[49]  E. Macaluso,et al.  Neural basis for priming of pop-out during visual search revealed with fMRI. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  Simon Baron-Cohen,et al.  Finding a face in the crowd: Testing the anger superiority effect in Asperger Syndrome , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[51]  Michael S. C. Thomas,et al.  Exploring the Williams syndrome face-processing debate: the importance of building developmental trajectories. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[52]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  People with Williams syndrome process faces holistically , 2003, Cognition.

[53]  W. Yule,et al.  A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. , 1991, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[54]  M. Rutherford,et al.  A threat-detection advantage in those with autism spectrum disorders , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[55]  C. Mervis,et al.  Neural correlates of genetically abnormal social cognition in Williams syndrome , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[56]  M Preus,et al.  The Williams syndrome: objective definition and diagnosis , 1984, Clinical genetics.

[57]  T. Jenkin,et al.  Older adults respond quickly to angry faces despite labeling difficulty. , 2009, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[58]  Lawrence G. Appelbaum,et al.  III. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Processing in Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[59]  L. F. Barrett,et al.  Handbook of Emotions , 1993 .

[60]  Brian W. Haas,et al.  Genetic Influences on Sociability: Heightened Amygdala Reactivity and Event-Related Responses to Positive Social Stimuli in Williams Syndrome , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  J. Eastwood,et al.  Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[62]  Michael W. Spratling,et al.  Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams Syndrome , 2001, Neuroreport.

[63]  Arne Öhman,et al.  The Malicious Serpent , 2003 .

[64]  R. Adolphs,et al.  II. Hypersociability in Williams Syndrome , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[65]  Peter J. B. Hancock,et al.  Viewing it differently: Social scene perception in Williams syndrome and Autism , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[66]  D. Purcell,et al.  It Takes a Confounded Face to Pop Out of a Crowd , 1996, Perception.

[67]  A. Young,et al.  Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[68]  C. H. Hansen,et al.  Finding the face in the crowd: an anger superiority effect. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  Francesco Fera,et al.  The Amygdala Response to Emotional Stimuli: A Comparison of Faces and Scenes , 2002, NeuroImage.

[70]  Carolyn B. Mervis,et al.  The Williams Syndrome Cognitive Profile , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[71]  Arne Öhman,et al.  Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms. , 1993 .

[72]  H. Tager-Flusberg,et al.  Model syndromes for investigating social cognitive and affective neuroscience: a comparison of Autism and Williams syndrome. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[73]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Verbal and nonverbal abilities in the Williams syndrome phenotype: evidence for diverging developmental trajectories. , 1998, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[74]  S. Rauch,et al.  Response and Habituation of the Human Amygdala during Visual Processing of Facial Expression , 1996, Neuron.

[75]  Ursula Bellugi,et al.  I. The Neurocognitive Profile of Williams Syndrome: A Complex Pattern of Strengths and Weaknesses , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  C. Mervis,et al.  Neural Basis of Genetically Determined Visuospatial Construction Deficit in Williams Syndrome , 2004, Neuron.

[77]  C. Darwin,et al.  The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , 1872 .