Surprise attracts the eyes and binds the gaze
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Cave,et al. Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] W. Johnston,et al. Novel popout is an attention-based phenomenon: An ERP analysis , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] G Horstmann,et al. [Effect of associative strength of schema elements on intensity of startle reaction]. , 1998, Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Psychologie : Organ der Deutschen Gesellschaft fur Psychologie.
[4] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. Novelty and saliency in attentional capture by unannounced motion singletons. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[5] G. Horstmann. Attentional capture by an unannounced color singleton depends on expectation discrepancy. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[7] Christopher L Asplund,et al. Surprise-induced blindness: a stimulus-driven attentional limit to conscious perception. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] Ulrich Ansorge,et al. Top-down contingent feature-specific orienting with and without awareness of the visual input , 2011, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[9] Gernot Horstmann,et al. The time course of intended and unintended allocation of attention , 2006, Psychological research.
[10] J. Henderson,et al. Does gravity matter? Effects of semantic and syntactic inconsistencies on the allocation of attention during scene perception. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[11] A. Schützwohl,et al. Surprise and schema strength , 1998 .
[12] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[13] J. C. Johnston,et al. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] S. Yantis,et al. On the distinction between visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[15] H. Egeth,et al. Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] P. Cavanagh,et al. Predictive remapping of attention across eye movements , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[17] Stefanie I. Becker,et al. Evidence for goal-independent attentional capture from validity effects with unexpected novel color cues—A response to Burnham (2007) , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[18] E Levonian,et al. Retention over time in relation to arousal during learning: an explantation of discrepant results. , 1972, Acta psychologica.
[19] Gernot Horstmann,et al. Evidence for Attentional Capture by a Surprising Color Singleton in Visual Search , 2002, Psychological science.
[20] G. Mangun,et al. Electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies of voluntary and reflexive attention. , 2000 .
[21] Udo Rudolph,et al. An experimental analysis of surprise , 1991 .
[22] J. Theeuwes. Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[23] H. Deubel,et al. Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.
[24] B. Gibson,et al. Surprise! An Unexpected Color Singleton Does Not Capture Attention in Visual Search , 1998 .
[25] W. Meyer,et al. Toward a Process Analysis of Emotions: The Case of Surprise , 1997 .
[26] Bryan R. Burnham,et al. Displaywide visual features associated with a search display’s appearance can mediate attentional capture , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[27] J. Theeuwes,et al. On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .
[28] J. Theeuwes. Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.