Once an optimist, always an optimist? Studying cognitive judgment bias in mice

Abstract Individuals differ in the way they judge ambiguous information: some individuals interpret ambiguous information in a more optimistic, and others in a more pessimistic way. Over the past two decades, such “optimistic” and “pessimistic” cognitive judgment biases (CJBs) have been utilized in animal welfare science as indicators of animals’ emotional states. However, empirical studies on their ecological and evolutionary relevance are still lacking. We, therefore, aimed at transferring the concept of “optimism” and “pessimism” to behavioral ecology and investigated the role of genetic and environmental factors in modulating CJB in mice. In addition, we assessed the temporal stability of individual differences in CJB. We show that the chosen genotypes (C57BL/6J and B6D2F1N) and environments (“scarce” and “complex”) did not have a statistically significant influence on the responses in the CJB test. By contrast, they influenced anxiety-like behavior with C57BL/6J mice and mice from the “complex” environment displaying less anxiety-like behavior than B6D2F1N mice and mice from the “scarce” environment. As the selected genotypes and environments did not explain the existing differences in CJB, future studies might investigate the impact of other genotypes and environmental conditions on CJB, and additionally, elucidate the role of other potential causes like endocrine profiles and epigenetic modifications. Furthermore, we show that individual differences in CJB were repeatable over a period of seven weeks, suggesting that CJB represents a temporally stable trait in laboratory mice. Therefore, we encourage the further study of CJB within an animal personality framework.

[1]  D. Weary,et al.  Negative expectations and vulnerability to stressors in animals , 2021, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews.

[2]  S. H. Richter,et al.  Regular touchscreen training affects faecal corticosterone metabolites and anxiety-like behaviour in mice , 2020, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  M. Mendl,et al.  Emotion in animal contests , 2020, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

[4]  Natasha A. Karp,et al.  Improving reproducibility in animal research by splitting the study population into several ‘mini-experiments’ , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[5]  S. H. Richter,et al.  Effects of different social experiences on emotional state in mice , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[6]  N. Dingemanse,et al.  Criteria for acceptable studies of animal personality and behavioural syndromes , 2020 .

[7]  Heiko G. Rödel,et al.  More exploratory house mice judge an ambiguous situation more negatively , 2020, Animal Cognition.

[8]  M. Bateson,et al.  Optimism, pessimism and judgement bias in animals: A systematic review and meta-analysis , 2020, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[9]  N. Dingemanse,et al.  Robustness of linear mixed‐effects models to violations of distributional assumptions , 2020, Methods in Ecology and Evolution.

[10]  M. Mendl,et al.  Animal affect and decision-making , 2020, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[11]  M. Bateson,et al.  Pharmacological manipulations of judgement bias: A systematic review and meta-analysis , 2019, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[12]  S. H. Richter,et al.  The Effects of Different Feeding Routines on Welfare in Laboratory Mice , 2020, Frontiers in Veterinary Science.

[13]  S. H. Richter,et al.  Differential Effects of Serotonin Transporter Genotype on Anxiety-Like Behavior and Cognitive Judgment Bias in Mice , 2019, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[14]  M. Špinka,et al.  Pair housing makes calves more optimistic , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[15]  François‐Xavier Dechaume‐Moncharmont,et al.  Pair-bonding influences affective state in a monogamous fish species , 2019, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

[16]  Carina Bodden,et al.  Heterogenising study samples across testing time improves reproducibility of behavioural data , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[17]  L. Kendall,et al.  Physiologic Stress of Ear Punch Identification Compared with Restraint Only in Mice. , 2019, Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science : JAALAS.

[18]  S. H. Richter,et al.  Technology or ecology? New tools to assess cognitive judgement bias in mice , 2019, Behavioural Brain Research.

[19]  N. Lazar,et al.  Moving to a World Beyond “p < 0.05” , 2019, The American Statistician.

[20]  O. N. Tikhodeyev,et al.  The Problem of Non-Shared Environment in Behavioral Genetics , 2019, Behavior Genetics.

[21]  D. Weary,et al.  Dairy calves’ personality traits predict social proximity and response to an emotional challenge , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[22]  B. Voelkl,et al.  Effects of Cage Enrichment on Behavior, Welfare and Outcome Variability in Female Mice , 2018, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[23]  H. Løvlie,et al.  Heritabilities and co-variation among cognitive traits in red junglefowl , 2018, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  A cross-species judgement bias task: integrating active trial initiation into a spatial Go/No-go task , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[25]  D. Weary,et al.  Pessimism and fearfulness in dairy calves , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[26]  L. Gygax,et al.  Mood As Cumulative Expectation Mismatch: A Test of Theory Based on Data from Non-verbal Cognitive Bias Tests , 2017, Front. Psychol..

[27]  Per B. Brockhoff,et al.  lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models , 2017 .

[28]  Martin A. Stoffel,et al.  rptR: repeatability estimation and variance decomposition by generalized linear mixed‐effects models , 2017 .

[29]  Heiko G. Rödel,et al.  Bottlenose dolphins engaging in more social affiliative behaviour judge ambiguous cues more optimistically , 2017, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  Kelly Gouveia,et al.  Optimising reliability of mouse performance in behavioural testing: the major role of non-aversive handling , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[31]  F. Péron,et al.  You know what? I’m happy. Cognitive bias is not related to personality but is induced by pair-housing in canaries (Serinus canaria) , 2017, Behavioural Processes.

[32]  M. Bateson Optimistic and pessimistic biases: a primer for behavioural ecologists , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[33]  A. Basu,et al.  Effects of environmental enrichment on anxiety-like behavior, sociability, sensory gating, and spatial learning in male and female C57BL/6J mice , 2016, Behavioural Brain Research.

[34]  L. Collins,et al.  Mood and personality interact to determine cognitive biases in pigs , 2016, Biology Letters.

[35]  F. J. van der Staay,et al.  Making Decisions under Ambiguity: Judgment Bias Tasks for Assessing Emotional State in Animals , 2016, Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience.

[36]  H. Würbel,et al.  Effect of Cage-Induced Stereotypies on Measures of Affective State and Recurrent Perseveration in CD-1 and C57BL/6 Mice , 2016, PloS one.

[37]  S. H. Richter,et al.  Daily exposure to a touchscreen-paradigm and associated food restriction evokes an increase in adrenocortical and neural activity in mice , 2016, Hormones and Behavior.

[38]  N. Lazar,et al.  The ASA Statement on p-Values: Context, Process, and Purpose , 2016 .

[39]  A. Bassols,et al.  Effect of gender and halothane genotype on cognitive bias and its relationship with fear in pigs , 2016 .

[40]  C. Krahé,et al.  Resolving Ambiguity in Emotional Disorders: The Nature and Role of Interpretation Biases. , 2016, Annual review of clinical psychology.

[41]  Y. Niv,et al.  Mood as Representation of Momentum , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  S. Reese,et al.  Pasturing in herds after housing in horseboxes induces a positive cognitive bias in horses , 2016 .

[43]  R. Prediger,et al.  Influence of environmental enrichment vs. time-of-day on behavioral repertoire of male albino Swiss mice , 2015, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[44]  E. Bethell A “How-To” Guide for Designing Judgment Bias Studies to Assess Captive Animal Welfare , 2015, Journal of applied animal welfare science : JAAWS.

[45]  M. Bateson,et al.  Opposite Effects of Early-Life Competition and Developmental Telomere Attrition on Cognitive Biases in Juvenile European Starlings , 2015, PloS one.

[46]  Rui F. Oliveira,et al.  Linking appraisal to behavioral flexibility in animals: implications for stress research , 2015, Front. Behav. Neurosci..

[47]  Flávia Teixeira-Silva,et al.  Environmental enrichment for adult rats: Effects on trait and state anxiety , 2015, Neuroscience Letters.

[48]  L. Gygax The A to Z of statistics for testing cognitive judgement bias , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[49]  Rebecca S. Schreiber,et al.  Hope for the Best or Prepare for the Worst? Towards a Spatial Cognitive Bias Test for Mice , 2014, PloS one.

[50]  D. Bates,et al.  Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4 , 2014, 1406.5823.

[51]  D. Weary,et al.  Separation from the Dam Causes Negative Judgement Bias in Dairy Calves , 2014, PloS one.

[52]  Repeated exposure to positive events induces optimistic-like judgment and enhances fearfulness in chronically stressed sheep , 2014 .

[53]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  The evolution of decision rules in complex environments , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[55]  E. Fuchs,et al.  Chronic psychosocial stress makes rats more ‘pessimistic’ in the ambiguous-cue interpretation paradigm , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[56]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  On the Evolution and Optimality of Mood States , 2013, Behavioral sciences.

[57]  K. Lesch,et al.  Unexpected effects of early-life adversity and social enrichment on the anxiety profile of mice varying in serotonin transporter genotype , 2013, Behavioural Brain Research.

[58]  P. Popik,et al.  Trait Pessimism Predicts Vulnerability to Stress-Induced Anhedonia in Rats , 2013, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[59]  Melanie Dammhahn,et al.  Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness , 2012, Animal Behaviour.

[60]  S. H. Richter,et al.  A glass full of optimism: Enrichment effects on cognitive bias in a rat model of depression , 2012, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[61]  atherine Douglasa,et al.  nvironmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive biases in pigs , 2012 .

[62]  L. Lewejohann,et al.  "Personality" in laboratory mice used for biomedical research: a way of understanding variability? , 2011, Developmental psychobiology.

[63]  M. Bateson,et al.  Agitated Honeybees Exhibit Pessimistic Cognitive Biases , 2011, Current Biology.

[64]  N. Brydges,et al.  Environmental enrichment induces optimistic cognitive bias in rats , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[65]  A. Houston,et al.  Environmental variability can select for optimism or pessimism. , 2011, Ecology letters.

[66]  N. Dingemanse,et al.  Evolutionary and ecological approaches to the study of personality , 2010, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[67]  A. Weiss,et al.  Animal personality , 2010, Current Biology.

[68]  Shinichi Nakagawa,et al.  Repeatability for Gaussian and non‐Gaussian data: a practical guide for biologists , 2010, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[69]  M. Mendl,et al.  An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[70]  G. Mason,et al.  Frustration and perseveration in stereotypic captive animals: Is a taste of enrichment worse than none at all? , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[71]  R. Spanagel,et al.  Stress triggers anhedonia in rats bred for learned helplessness , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[72]  Oliver von Bohlen und Halbach,et al.  Ambiguous-Cue Interpretation is Biased Under Stress- and Depression-Like States in Rats , 2010, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[73]  H. Schielzeth Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients , 2010 .

[74]  Denis Réale,et al.  Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual plasticity. , 2010, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[75]  A. Boissy,et al.  Release from restraint generates a positive judgement bias in sheep , 2010 .

[76]  E. Erdfelder,et al.  Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses , 2009, Behavior research methods.

[77]  Hadley Wickham,et al.  ggplot2 - Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis (2nd Edition) , 2017 .

[78]  Kate L. Laskowski,et al.  The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[79]  Sébastien Lê,et al.  FactoMineR: An R Package for Multivariate Analysis , 2008 .

[80]  Melissa Bateson,et al.  Larger, enriched cages are associated with 'optimistic' response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) , 2008 .

[81]  P. McGuffin,et al.  In the Face of Uncertainty: A Twin Study of Ambiguous Information, Anxiety and Depression in Children , 2008, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[82]  N. Dingemanse,et al.  Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution , 2007, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.

[83]  M. Bashaw,et al.  Maximizing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment: Suggestions from the experimental analysis of behavior , 2007 .

[84]  M. Bateson,et al.  Performance on a categorisation task suggests that removal of environmental enrichment induces ‘pessimism’ in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) , 2007, Animal Welfare.

[85]  K. Frick,et al.  Long-term continuous, but not daily, environmental enrichment reduces spatial memory decline in aged male mice , 2006, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[86]  Guy M McKhann,et al.  Hippocampal Neurogenesis is not Required for Behavioral Effects of Environmental Enrichment. , 2006, Neurosurgery.

[87]  M. Mendl,et al.  Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[88]  T. Groothuis,et al.  Avian personalities: characterization and epigenesis , 2005, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[89]  H. Rauvala,et al.  Long‐term individual housing in C57BL/6J and DBA/2 mice: assessment of behavioral consequences , 2004, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[90]  I. Yaniv,et al.  Environmental enrichment in mice decreases anxiety, attenuates stress responses and enhances natural killer cell activity , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[91]  Sasha R. X. Dall,et al.  The behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective , 2004 .

[92]  Andrew Sih,et al.  Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. , 2004, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[93]  R. Lister The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[94]  P. Gard,et al.  Strain differences in the anxiolytic effects of losartan in the mouse , 2001, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[95]  G. Griebel,et al.  The free‐exploratory paradigm: an effective method for measuring neophobic behaviour in mice and testing potential neophobia‐reducing drugs , 1993, Behavioural pharmacology.

[96]  R. Lister Ethologically-based animal models of anxiety disorders. , 1990, Pharmacology & therapeutics.

[97]  J. Wehner,et al.  Inheritance of spatial learning ability in inbred mice: a classical genetic analysis. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[98]  D. Treit,et al.  Thigmotaxis as a test for anxiolytic activity in rats , 1988, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[99]  S. File,et al.  Validation of open : closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat , 1985, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[100]  J. Archer Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: a review. , 1973, Animal behaviour.

[101]  F. James Rohlf,et al.  Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research , 1969 .

[102]  S. Shapiro,et al.  An Analysis of Variance Test for Normality (Complete Samples) , 1965 .