Risk perception, road behavior, and pedestrian injury among adolescent students in Kathmandu, Nepal

Objective: To examine the relationship between the perceived safety of specified road behaviors, self-described road behaviors, and pedestrian injury among adolescent students in Kathmandu, Nepal. Methods: A cross-sectional study was conducted among 1557 adolescents in grades 6–8 across 14 schools in Kathmandu using a self-administered questionnaire in 2003. A multiple logistic regression analysis was used to analyze the data. Results: Adolescents were more likely to suffer from pedestrian injury when they did not always “wait for green signals to cross the road”. There were no significant associations between road behaviors such as “looking both ways along the road before crossing” or “playing in the road or sidewalks” and pedestrian injury. Adolescents who “perceived it safe to cross the road from any point” or “did not perceive it to be safer to cross the road at a zebra crossing” were less likely to “look both ways” or “wait for green signals” before crossing the road. Adolescents who “perceived it to be safe to play in the road” were more likely to play in the road or sidewalk. Similarly, this study showed a positive association between road safety education and adolescents’ road crossing behaviors. Conclusion: Adolescents’ road behaviors, except for compliance with green signals, were not significantly associated with pedestrian injury. This suggests that a behavioral approach without modification of the traffic environment (such as provision of crossing signals) might not effectively prevent the occurrence of pedestrian injury in developing countries with poor traffic conditions.

[1]  M. Becker,et al.  The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later , 1984, Health education quarterly.

[2]  F K Winston,et al.  Exposure to traffic among urban children injured as pedestrians , 2002, Injury prevention : journal of the International Society for Child and Adolescent Injury Prevention.

[3]  D Cross,et al.  Child pedestrian injury prevention project: student results. , 2000, Preventive medicine.

[4]  R. Norton,et al.  Effect of environmental factors on risk of injury of child pedestrians by motor vehicles: a case-control study , 1995, BMJ.

[5]  M R Stevenson,et al.  The validity of children's self-reported exposure to traffic. , 1996, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[6]  M. Joly,et al.  Geographical and socio-ecological variations of traffic accidents among children. , 1991, Social science & medicine.

[7]  A. Kriska,et al.  The epidemiology of leisure physical activity in an adolescent population. , 1993, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[8]  M Cameron,et al.  World Report on Road Traffic Injury Prevention. , 2004 .

[9]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Tracking multiple targets with multifocal attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  M. Reich,et al.  The neglected epidemic: road traffic injuries in developing countries , 2002, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[11]  M Suzanne Zeedyk,et al.  Stop, look, listen, and think? What young children really do when crossing the road. , 2002, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[12]  I. Rosenstock The Health Belief Model and Preventive Health Behavior , 1974 .

[13]  L. Laflamme,et al.  Injuries among children in Karachi, Pakistan--what, where and how. , 2004, Public health.

[14]  Masao Ichikawa,et al.  Relationship between mechanisms and activities at the time of pedestrian injury and activity limitation among school adolescents in Kathmandu, Nepal. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[15]  Margaret M. Peden,et al.  World Report on Road Traffic Injury Prevention , 2004 .

[16]  A. Butchart,et al.  Perceptions of injury causes and solutions in a Johannesburg township: implications for prevention. , 2000, Social science & medicine.

[17]  H Chotani,et al.  Pedestrian environment and behavior in Karachi, Pakistan. , 1999, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[18]  K. Mukhida,et al.  Pediatric neurotrauma in Kathmandu, Nepal: implications for injury management and control , 2006, Child's Nervous System.

[19]  A. Joshi,et al.  Unintentional injuries among school adolescents in Kathmandu, Nepal: a descriptive study. , 2006, Public health.

[20]  C Mock,et al.  The effect of recall on estimation of incidence rates for injury in Ghana. , 1999, International journal of epidemiology.

[21]  R B Isler,et al.  Child pedestrians' crossing gap thresholds. , 1998, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[22]  Ivar Heuch,et al.  Perceived susceptibility to and perceived causes of road traffic injuries in an urban and rural area of Tanzania. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[23]  P. Scheidt,et al.  The effects of recall on estimating annual nonfatal injury rates for children and adolescents. , 1994, American journal of public health.

[24]  B. Morrongiello Children's perspectives on injury and close-call experiences: sex differences in injury-outcome processes. , 1997, Journal of pediatric psychology.

[25]  John van der Kamp,et al.  Visual timing and adaptive behavior in a road-crossing simulation study. , 2005, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  T. Pitcairn,et al.  Individual differences in road crossing ability in young children and adults. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[27]  Y. Holder,et al.  Injury surveillance guidelines , 2001 .