Journal of Experimental

Children with poor arithmetic fact mastery (n=45) at the end of third grade were compared to grade-level peers with good arithmetic fact mastery (n=60) in competencies related to reading and mathematics. Children were assessed longitudinally across second and third grades. When predictor variables such as IQ were held constant, the poor fact mastery and good fact mastery groups performed at about the same level and progressed at a comparable rate on math story problems and on broad reading achievement. The groups also progressed at a comparable rate on broad math achievement, although children with poor fact mastery performed at a significantly lower level. Children with poor fact mastery showed remarkably little growth on timed number facts during the study period, despite normal growth in other areas of mathematics. Deficits in fact mastery are highly persistent and appear to be independent of reading and language abilities.

[1]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. , 2003, Child development.

[2]  T. R. Miles,et al.  Dyslexia, the pattern of difficulties , 1983 .

[3]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Cognitive Arithmetic and Problem Solving , 1997, Journal of learning disabilities.

[4]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Performance across different areas of mathematical cognition in children with learning difficulties. , 2001 .

[5]  M. Ashcraft Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.

[6]  R. Johnston,et al.  Children's arithmetical difficulties: contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term memory. , 1997, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[7]  John B. Willett,et al.  Using covariance structure analysis to detect correlates and predictors of individual change over time , 1994 .

[8]  James G. Greeno,et al.  Developmental analysis of understanding language about quantities and of solving problems. , 1988 .

[9]  David C. Geary,et al.  Children's Mathematical Development: Research and Practical Applications , 1996 .

[10]  Linda M. Collins,et al.  Best Methods for the Analysis of Change: Recent Advances, Unanswered Questions, Future Directions , 1991 .

[11]  Carole S. Robinson,et al.  Toward a Two–Factor Theory of One Type of Mathematics Disabilities , 2002 .

[12]  John B. Willett,et al.  Questions and Answers in the Measurement of Change , 1988 .

[13]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Language and calculation within the parietal lobe: a combined cognitive, anatomical and fMRI study , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  A. Goldberger,et al.  Structural Equation Models in the Social Sciences. , 1974 .

[15]  D. Geary Mathematical disabilities: cognitive, neuropsychological, and genetic components. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[16]  Snorre A. Ostad,et al.  Developmental progression of subtraction strategies : a comparison of mathematically normal and mathematically disabled children , 1999 .

[17]  Mary S. Riley,et al.  Development of Children's Problem-Solving Ability in Arithmetic. , 1984 .

[18]  David Rogosa,et al.  A growth curve approach to the measurement of change. , 1982 .

[19]  H. F. Gollob,et al.  Interpreting and estimating indirect effects assuming time lags really matter. , 1991 .

[20]  Herbert P. Ginsburg,et al.  The development of mathematical thinking , 1983 .

[21]  Snorre A. Ostad,et al.  Developmental differences in addition strategies: a comparison of mathematically disabled and mathematically normal children. , 1997, The British journal of educational psychology.

[22]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Numerical and arithmetical cognition: a longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[23]  Harry P. Bahrick,et al.  Lifetime maintenance of high school mathematics content. , 1991 .

[24]  J. Dixon,et al.  Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 2001 .

[25]  C. A. Rashotte,et al.  Test of Word Reading Efficiency , 1999 .

[26]  William Meredith,et al.  Chapter 4 – Longitudinal Factor Analysis , 1990 .

[27]  Nancy C. Jordan,et al.  Mathematical Thinking in Second-Grade Children with Different Forms of LD , 2000, Journal of learning disabilities.

[28]  James M. Moser,et al.  The Acquisition of Addition and Subtraction Concepts in Grades One through Three. , 1984 .

[29]  R. Siegler,et al.  Conscious and unconscious strategy discoveries: a microgenetic analysis. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[30]  Mary K. Hoard,et al.  Numerical and arithmetical cognition: patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[31]  Arthur J. Baroody,et al.  Children's Relational Knowledge of Addition and Subtraction , 1999 .

[32]  K. Jöreskog A General Method for Estimating a Linear Structural Equation System. , 1970 .

[33]  Anthony S. Bryk,et al.  Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods , 1992 .

[34]  Eric A. Jenkins,et al.  How Children Discover New Strategies , 1989 .

[35]  Herbert P. Ginsburg,et al.  Cognitive Analysis of Children's Mathematics Difficulties , 1984 .