Dorsal hippocampus, CA3, and CA1 lesions disrupt temporal sequence completion.

An experiment was designed to evaluate effects of dorsal hippocampus, dorsal CA3a,b, dorsal CA1, and control lesions on performance of a temporal sequence task. Rats were trained on a sequential learning task involving six spatial locations on a radial 8-arm maze. After initial training followed by surgery, it was found that all lesioned animals were able to remember the sequence. To test temporal sequence completion, rats were started at different positions in the sequence and expected to complete the remainder of the sequence. The results indicate that control rats had no difficulty completing the sequence, regardless of starting point. In contrast, rats with dorsal hippocampus and dorsal CA3a,b lesions made errors by always returning to the first position in the sequence, regardless of which start position was used, whereas rats with dorsal CA1 lesions made random errors in the process of completing the sequence and did not appear to remember the serial order of the spatial sequence. This suggests that the dorsal hippocampus, and specifically the dorsal CA3 in conjunction with CA1, may be involved in temporal pattern completion processes.

[1]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Hippocampal conjunctive encoding, storage, and recall: Avoiding a trade‐off , 1994, Hippocampus.

[2]  J. Lisman,et al.  Storage, recall, and novelty detection of sequences by the hippocampus: Elaborating on the SOCRATIC model to account for normal and aberrant effects of dopamine , 2001, Hippocampus.

[3]  J. Lisman,et al.  The Hippocampal-VTA Loop: Controlling the Entry of Information into Long-Term Memory , 2005, Neuron.

[4]  Marc W Howard,et al.  The temporal context model in spatial navigation and relational learning: toward a common explanation of medial temporal lobe function across domains. , 2005, Psychological review.

[5]  M. R. Mehta,et al.  Role of experience and oscillations in transforming a rate code into a temporal code , 2002, Nature.

[6]  J. Lisman Relating Hippocampal Circuitry to Function Recall of Memory Sequences by Reciprocal Dentate–CA3 Interactions , 1999, Neuron.

[7]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  The hippocampus as an associator of discontiguous events , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[8]  E. Rolls,et al.  Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory , 1994, Hippocampus.

[9]  W B Levy,et al.  A sequence predicting CA3 is a flexible associator that learns and uses context to solve hippocampal‐like tasks , 1996, Hippocampus.

[10]  D Marr,et al.  Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. , 1971, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  R. Kesner,et al.  A double dissociation between the rat hippocampus and medial caudoputamen in processing two forms of knowledge. , 2000, Behavioral neuroscience.

[12]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neural networks and brain function , 1998 .

[13]  M. Quirk,et al.  Requirement for Hippocampal CA3 NMDA Receptors in Associative Memory Recall , 2002, Science.

[14]  E. Rolls A theory of hippocampal function in memory , 1996, Hippocampus.

[15]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  A Behavioral Assessment of Hippocampal Function Based on a Subregional Analysis , 2004, Reviews in the neurosciences.

[16]  D. Norman Learning and Memory , 1982 .

[17]  M. Moser,et al.  Functional differentiation in the hippocampus , 1998, Hippocampus.

[18]  O Jensen,et al.  Theta/gamma networks with slow NMDA channels learn sequences and encode episodic memory: role of NMDA channels in recall. , 1996, Learning & memory.

[19]  E. Rolls,et al.  A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory , 2006, Progress in Neurobiology.