Where the wild things are: informal experience and ecological reasoning.

Category-based induction requires selective use of different relations to guide inferences; this article examines the development of inferences based on ecological relations among living things. Three hundred and forty-six 6-, 8-, and 10-year-old children from rural, suburban, and urban communities projected novel diseases or insides from one species to an ecologically or taxonomically related species; they were also surveyed about hobbies and activities. Frequency of ecological inferences increased with age and with reports of informal exploration of nature, and decreased with population density. By age 10, children preferred taxonomic inferences for insides and ecological inferences for disease, but this pattern emerged earlier among rural children. These results underscore the importance of context by demonstrating effects of both domain-relevant experience and environment on biological reasoning.

[1]  E. Heit,et al.  Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[2]  D. Medin,et al.  A relevance theory of induction , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[3]  Jennifer A. Joy-Gaba,et al.  Cognitive “Category-Based Induction” Research and Social “Persuasion” Research Are Each About What Makes Arguments Believable , 2010, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[4]  E. Markman,et al.  Categories and induction in young children , 1986, Cognition.

[5]  D L Medin,et al.  Expertise and category-based induction. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[6]  F. E. Kuo,et al.  Social Aspects of Urban Forestry: the Role of Arboriculture in a Healthy Social Ecology , 2003 .

[7]  S. Gelman,et al.  On wooden pillows: multiple classification and children's category-based inductions. , 1992, Child development.

[8]  D. Medin,et al.  Human-centeredness is Not a Universal Feature of Young Children's Reasoning: Culture and Experience Matter When Reasoning About Biological Entities. , 2010, Cognitive development.

[9]  Evan Heit,et al.  Relations among categorization, induction, recognition, and similarity: comment on Sloutsky and Fisher (2004). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Gregory L Murphy,et al.  An apple is more than just a fruit: cross-classification in children's concepts. , 2003, Child development.

[11]  S. Sloman Feature-Based Induction , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  Stephen Kaplan,et al.  The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework , 1995 .

[13]  Do development and learning really decrease memory? On similarity and category-based induction in adults and children , 2008, Cognition.

[14]  S. Levinson,et al.  WEIRD languages have misled us, too , 2010, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[15]  Andrzej Tarłowski If it's an animal it has axons: Experience and culture in preschool children's reasoning about animates , 2006 .

[16]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  The Tree of Life: Universal and Cultural Features of Folkbiological Taxonomies and Inductions , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[17]  D. Medin,et al.  Folkbiological reasoning from a cross-cultural developmental perspective: early essentialist notions are shaped by cultural beliefs. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[18]  Nadya Vasilyeva,et al.  Chapter 5 - Generating Inductive Inferences: Premise Relations and Property Effects , 2010 .

[19]  Patrick Shafto,et al.  Effects of time pressure on context-sensitive property induction , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[20]  E. Heit,et al.  Inductive reasoning , 1992, Language Computations.

[21]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Inductive reasoning about causally transmitted properties , 2008, Cognition.

[22]  Patrick Shafto,et al.  Development of categorization and reasoning in the natural world: novices to experts, naive similarity to ecological knowledge. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  V. Sloutsky,et al.  Induction and categorization in young children: a similarity-based model. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Category-Based Induction , 1990 .

[25]  C. B. Colby The weirdest people in the world , 1973 .

[26]  Peter H. Kahn,et al.  The Human Relationship with Nature: Development and Culture , 1999 .

[27]  D. Medin,et al.  Cultural and experiential differences in the development of folkbiological induction , 2003 .

[28]  J. Coley,et al.  On the importance of comparative research: the case of folkbiology. , 2000, Child development.

[29]  Patrick Shafto,et al.  Knowledge and Category-Based Induction. , 2005 .

[30]  A. F. Taylor,et al.  Children and their Environments: Is contact with nature important for healthy child development? State of the evidence , 2006 .

[31]  D. Medin,et al.  Inductive reasoning in folkbiological thought. , 1999 .

[32]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[33]  Brian H. Ross,et al.  Food for Thought: Cross-Classification and Category Organization in a Complex Real-World Domain , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  L. Rips Inductive judgments about natural categories. , 1975 .

[35]  G. Murphy,et al.  Thematic relations in adults' concepts. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[36]  Kayoko Inagaki,et al.  The effects of raising animals on children's biological knowledge , 1990 .

[37]  E. Hunn The Native Mind and the Cultural Construction of Nature , 2010 .